Social Cognitive Theory

Chapter 13

SOCIAL-COGNITIVE THEORY: APPLICATIONS, RELATED THEORETICAL CONCEPTIONS, AND CONTEMPORARY RESEARCH

1

Save your time - order a paper!

Get your paper written from scratch within the tight deadline. Our service is a reliable solution to all your troubles. Place an order on any task and we will take care of it. You won’t have to worry about the quality and deadlines

Order Paper Now

© 2018 John Wiley & Sons, Inc. This presentation may be used and adapted for use in classes using the fourteenth edition of Personality. It may not be re-distributed except to students enrolled in such classes and in such case must be password protected to limit access to students enrolled in such classes. Students may not re-distribute portions of the original presentation.

 

QUESTIONS TO BE ADDRESSED IN THIS CHAPTER

How do knowledge structures – especially cognitive “schemas” – contribute to personality functioning and help to explain individual differences?

How do personal goals and standards of self-evaluation differ from one person to another, and how do these differences relate to motivation and emotional life?

What is the role of self-efficacy beliefs and other self-referent thinking processes in psychological disorders and therapeutic change?

What are some scientific challenges that were not addressed in the original formulations of social-cognitive theory and how have they been addressed by contemporary developments in personality theory?

COGNITIVE COMPONENTS OF PERSONALITY: BELIEFS, GOALS, AND EVALUATIVE STANDARDS

BELIEFS ABOUT THE SELF AND SELF-SCHEMAS

Schemas: knowledge structures that guide and organize the processing of info

Example: new song on the radio sounds structured because one has acquired schemas for song structures

Schemas guide one’s interpretation of the sounds that comprise the song

Music from a different culture might sound chaotic!

COGNITIVE COMPONENTS OF PERSONALITY: BELIEFS, GOALS, AND EVALUATIVE STANDARDS

BELIEFS ABOUT THE SELF AND SELF-SCHEMAS

Markus (1977) : many of our most important schemas concern ourselves

People form cognitive generalizations about the self just as they do about other things

Different people develop different self-schemas

Self-schemas may account for the relatively unique ways in which idiosyncratic individuals think about the world around them

COGNITIVE COMPONENTS OF PERSONALITY: BELIEFS, GOALS, AND EVALUATIVE STANDARDS

BELIEFS ABOUT THE SELF AND SELF-SCHEMAS

Self-Schemas and Reaction-Time Methods

Reaction-time measures: experimental methods in which an experimenter records not only the content of a person’s response, but also how long it takes the person to respond

People who possess a self-schema with regard to a given domain of social life should be faster in responding to questions regarding that life domain

COGNITIVE COMPONENTS OF PERSONALITY: BELIEFS, GOALS, AND EVALUATIVE STANDARDS

BELIEFS ABOUT THE SELF AND SELF-SCHEMAS

Self-Schemas and Reaction-Time Methods

Markus (1977) identified people who possessed a self-schema regarding independence

Participants rated themselves as high or low on independence

Participants indicated the degree to which the personality characteristic was important to them

Those who had an extreme high or low self-rating and thought independence/dependence was important were judged as schematic

Participants then asked to rate whether a series of adjectives (some of which were semantically related to independence/dependence) were descriptive of themselves

Schematics made these judgments faster

COGNITIVE COMPONENTS OF PERSONALITY: BELIEFS, GOALS, AND EVALUATIVE STANDARDS

BELIEFS ABOUT THE SELF AND SELF-SCHEMAS

Self-Schemas and Reaction-Time Methods

Andersen and Cyranowski (1994): women with differing sexual self-schemas would process interpersonal information differently and function differently in their sexual and romantic relationships

Women asked to rate themselves on a list of 50 adjectives, 26 of which were used to form a Sexual Self-Schema Scale (e.g., uninhibited, loving)

Asked to respond to measures that asked about sexual experiences and romantic involvement

COGNITIVE COMPONENTS OF PERSONALITY: BELIEFS, GOALS, AND EVALUATIVE STANDARDS

BELIEFS ABOUT THE SELF AND SELF-SCHEMAS

Self-Schemas and Reaction-Time Methods

Andersen and Cyranowski found that women with high scores on the Sexual Self-Schema Scale (particularly those with positive sexual self-schemas)

Were more sexually active

Experienced greater sexual arousal and sexual pleasure

Were more able to be involved in romantic love relationships

“Co-schematics (women who had both positive and negative schemas)” found to experience

High levels of involvement with sexual partners

High levels of sexual anxiety

COGNITIVE COMPONENTS OF PERSONALITY: BELIEFS, GOALS, AND EVALUATIVE STANDARDS

BELIEFS ABOUT THE SELF AND SELF-SCHEMAS

Self-Schemas and Reaction-Time Methods

People tend to live complex lives in which they develop a number of different self-schemas

Different situations may cause different self-schemas to be part of the working self-concept: the subset of self-concept that is in working memory at any given time

Info about the self that is in consciousness, and guides behavior, at any given time changes dynamically as people interact with the ever-changing events of the social world

COGNITIVE COMPONENTS OF PERSONALITY: BELIEFS, GOALS, AND EVALUATIVE STANDARDS

BELIEFS ABOUT THE SELF AND SELF-SCHEMAS

Self-Based Motives and Motivated Information Processing

Self-schemas motivate people to process information in particular ways

People often are biased toward positive views of the self, which can be explained by positing a self-enhancement motive

People also may be motivated to experience themselves as being consistent and predictable, reflecting a self-verification motive

COGNITIVE COMPONENTS OF PERSONALITY: BELIEFS, GOALS, AND EVALUATIVE STANDARDS

BELIEFS ABOUT THE SELF AND SELF-SCHEMAS

Self-Based Motives and Motivated Information Processing

What happens when the two motives conflict?

Evidence suggests we generally prefer positive feedback but prefer negative feedback in relation to negative self-views

Positive life events can be bad for one’s health if they conflict with a negative self-concept and disrupt one’s negative identity

There are individual differences in this regard

We may be more oriented toward self-enhancement in some relationships and self-verification in other relationships

CURRENT APPLICATIONS

SELF-SCHEMAS AND HISTORY OF SEXUAL ABUSE

Meston, Rellini, and Heiman (2006) hypothesized that abuse experiences may alter self-schemas and do so in a long-lasting manner

Conducted a study whose participants were 48 women with a history of child sexual abuse

Also studied a group of 71 women who had not suffered from abuse experiences and who thus served as control participants.

To measure sexual self-schemas, Meston et al. administered the sexual self-schema scale in which people report on their perceptions of their own sexuality

Women with a history of abuse believed themselves to be less romantic and passionate; that is, they had lower scores on the romantic/passionate items of the sexual self-schema measure

Women who had experienced abuse years earlier had more negative emotional experiences in the present day

Women with lower romantic/passionate self-schemas reported more negative emotional experiences

COGNITIVE COMPONENTS OF PERSONALITY: BELIEFS, GOALS, AND EVALUATIVE STANDARDS

LEARNING VERSUS PERFORMANCE GOALS

Different goals may lead to different patterns of thought, emotion, and behavior

Goals may be the cause of what one would interpret as different personality styles

Two ways of thinking about goals:

Learning goal: think about the task and all you can learn from it

Peformance goal: have the aim of

showing people how smart you are

avoiding embarrassment when you don’t know something

making a good impression

COGNITIVE COMPONENTS OF PERSONALITY: BELIEFS, GOALS, AND EVALUATIVE STANDARDS

LEARNING VERSUS PERFORMANCE GOALS

Elliott and Dweck (1988) induced learning versus performance goals among grade school students performing a cognitive task

Some told that they were performing a task that would sharpen mental skills

Others told they were performing a task that would be evaluated by experts

Students’ beliefs in their ability on the task (i.e., their efficacy beliefs) were also manipulated

People who had a combination of performance goals and low beliefs in their ability were less likely than others to develop useful strategies on the task

COGNITIVE COMPONENTS OF PERSONALITY: BELIEFS, GOALS, AND EVALUATIVE STANDARDS

LEARNING VERSUS PERFORMANCE GOALS

Elliott and Dweck (1988) recorded the degree to which people spontaneously expressed negative emotions while working on the task

Performance goal participants expressed much tension and anxiety when performing the task

“My stomach hurts” (Elliott & Dweck, 1988, p. 10)

Performance goals provides insight into what we commonly call “test anxiety”

Dweck’s social-cognitive analysis suggests that one might intervene by trying to change people’s patterns of thinking

COGNITIVE COMPONENTS OF PERSONALITY: BELIEFS, GOALS, AND EVALUATIVE STANDARDS

LEARNING VERSUS PERFORMANCE GOALS

Causes of Learning versus Performance Goals: Implicit Theories

Implicit theories: those we possess, that guide our thinking, but that we may not usually state in words

Implicit theories of interest to Dweck and colleagues: whether or not psychological attributes are changeable

Entity theory: a particular characteristic or trait is viewed as fixed

Incremental theory: a particular characteristic or trait is believed to be malleable or open to change

COGNITIVE COMPONENTS OF PERSONALITY: BELIEFS, GOALS, AND EVALUATIVE STANDARDS

LEARNING VERSUS PERFORMANCE GOALS

Causes of Learning versus Performance Goals: Implicit Theories

Children with an entity view of intelligence tend to set performance goals

If intelligence is fixed, then one interprets activities as a “performance” in which intelligence is evaluated

Children with an incremental view of intelligence tend to set learning goals

If intelligence can be increased, then natural to set the learning goal of acquiring experiences that increase it

COGNITIVE COMPONENTS OF PERSONALITY: BELIEFS, GOALS, AND EVALUATIVE STANDARDS

LEARNING VERSUS PERFORMANCE GOALS

Causes of Learning versus Performance Goals: Implicit Theories

Tamir, John, Srivastava, and Gross, 2007 study

Students about to enter college were tested about whether they believed emotions to be malleable and controllable vs. fixed and uncontrollable

As hypothesized, students with incremental (malleable) beliefs concerning emotion showed better emotion regulation than did those with entity (fixed) beliefs

Throughout the first term, relative to those with entity beliefs concerning emotion, those with incremental beliefs received increasing social support from new friends

By the end of the freshman year, those with incremental beliefs were found to have more positive moods and generally better levels of adjustment than those with entity beliefs

COGNITIVE COMPONENTS OF PERSONALITY: BELIEFS, GOALS, AND EVALUATIVE STANDARDS

LEARNING VERSUS PERFORMANCE GOALS

Causes of Learning versus Performance Goals: Implicit Theories

Blackwell, Trzesniewski, and Dweck (2007): If one could turn entity theorists into incremental theorists, one should be able to reduce test anxiety and boost performance

Enrolled 7th-graders in an educational intervention designed to induce an incremental theory of intelligence

Students learned that the human brain changes when people study, growing new connections among neurons that increase a person’s mental abilities (a separate group did not receive this instruction)

By the end of the year, students who had been exposed to the intervention began to outperform the other students

Personality and the Brain: Goals

Are goals and evaluative standards distinct biologically from other kinds of thoughts?

D’Argembeau et al. (2009) asked participants to imagine future outcomes that either were or were not personal goals for them

(e.g., Future doctors imagined becoming a doctor and going deep-sea fishing)

Participants were in a brain scanner while imagining these two types of outcomes.

Personality and the Brain: Goals

D’Argembeau et al. (2009), cont’d.

Two brain regions were more active when people thought about personal goals than about future activities that were not goals for them

Medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC)

Posterior cingulate cortex (PCC)

Why significant?

Personality and the Brain: Goals