Discussion: Integrating The Research Process

About 15 years ago, before evidence-based practice became a standard to which to aspire, a professor found an article describing a treatment program for substance-abusing African-American women that incorporated their children and aimed to preserve their families. The program was built around principles of African-centered theory. The professor thought the program appeared well designed and heartily urged students to study the article and possibly find ways to imitate the program ideas, if not the program itself. A few years later, the professor, having continued her education, especially in the area of research, searched for a follow-up on the program, perhaps reporting outcomes and evidence of effectiveness. No research evidence on the program appears in the scholarly journals. In fact, no evidence that the program continues to exist at this time has been found. The fact that what appeared to be a promising practice approach did not endure or have the opportunity to benefit from evaluation is unfortunate.

Note: This week you will work as a class to create a plan for evaluating the rite of passage program described in the assigned article. Each member of the class will be assigned one step of the research process:

1.Research problems and questions
2.Finding and using existing knowledge
3.Focused research questions and research hypothesis
4.Research methods
5.Sampling issues and options
6.Measurement
7.Data acquisition
8.Data collection instruments
9.Analyzing data
10.Disseminating research findings

To prepare for this Discussion, read the Poitier et al. study and then revisit what you have learned about your assigned step of the research process. Consider what would be the most important information to share with your colleagues about that step. Together the class will create a research plan for evaluating the rite of passage program described in the assigned article.

Required Readings

Poitier, V. L., Niliwaambieni, M., & Rowe, C. L. (1997). A rite of passage approach designed to preserve the families of substance-abusing African American women. Child Welfare76(1), 173–195.
Retrieved from Walden Library databases.

Briefly describe the types of professionals involved in various assessment processes.

DUE TOMORROW NO LATER…. 9/10/2018     NO PLAGIARISM.. EXAMPLE OF PAPER ATTACHED.

Psychological assessment guides are created by psychology professionals to provide the public with accurate and authoritative information appropriate for their current needs. Information available to the public about psychological testing and assessment varies widely depending on the professional creating it, the purpose of the assessment, and the intended audience. When professionals effectively educate the public on the howwhat, and why behind assessments and the strengths and limitations of commonly used instruments, potential clients are in a better position to be informed users of assessment products and services. The Assessment Guides developed in this course will be designed to provide the lay public with accurate and culturally relevant information to aid them in making informed decisions about psychological testing. Students will develop their Guides with the goal of educating readers to be informed participants in the assessment process.

There is no required template for the development of the Assessment Guide. Students are encouraged to be creative while maintaining the professional appearance of their work. The Guide must be reader-friendly (sixth- to ninth-grade reading level) and easy to navigate, and it must include a combination of text, images, and graphics to engage readers in the information provided. Throughout their Guides, students will provide useful examples and definitions as well as questions readers should ask their practitioners. To ensure accuracy, students are expected to use only scholarly and peer-reviewed sources for the information in the development of their Guides.

Students will begin their Guides with a general overview of assessment, reasons for assessment referrals, and the importance of the role of each individual in the process. Within each of the remaining sections, students will describe the types of assessments that their readers may encounter, the purposes of each type of assessment, the different skills and abilities the instruments measure, the most valid and reliable uses of the measures, and limitations of the measures. A brief section will be included to describe the assessment process, the types of professionals who conduct the assessments, and what to expect during the assessment meetings.

The Assessment Guide must include the following sections:

Table of Contents (Portrait orientation must be used for the page layout of this section.)
In this one-page section, students must list the following subsections and categories of assessments.

  • Introduction and Overview
  • Tests of Intelligence
  • Tests of Achievement
  • Tests of Ability
  • Neuropsychological Testing
  • Personality Testing
  • Industrial, Occupational, and Career Assessment
  • Forensic Assessment
  • Special Topics (student’s choice)
  • References

Section 1: Introduction and Overview (Portrait or landscape orientation may be used for the page layout of this section.)
Students will begin their Guides with a general overview of assessment. In this two-page section, students will briefly address the major aspects of the assessment process. Students are encouraged to develop creative titles for these topics that effectively communicate the meanings to the intended audience.

  • Definition of a Test (e.g., What is a Test?)
  • Briefly define psychological assessment.
  • Types of Tests
  • Identify the major categories of psychological assessment.
  • Reliability and Validity
  • Briefly define the concepts of reliability and validity as they apply to psychological assessment.
  • Role of testing and assessment in the diagnostic process
  • Briefly explain role of assessment in diagnosis.
  • Professionals Who Administer Tests
  • Briefly describe the types of professionals involved in various assessment processes.
  • Culture and Testing
  • Briefly describe issues of cultural diversity as it applies to psychological assessment.

Categories of Assessment (Portrait or landscape orientation may be used for the page layout of this section.)
For each of the following, students will create a two-page information sheet or pamphlet to be included in the Assessment Guide. For each category of assessment, students will include the required content listed in the PSY640 Content for Testing Pamphlets and Information Sheets (Links to an external site.)Links to an external site.. Be sure to reference the content requirements (Links to an external site.)Links to an external site. prior to completing each of the information sheets on the following categories of assessment.

  • Tests of Intelligence
  • Tests of Achievement
  • Tests of Ability
  • Neuropsychological Testing
  • Personality Testing
  • Industrial, Occupational, and Career Assessment
  • Forensic Assessment
  • Special Topics (Students will specify which topic they selected for this pamphlet or information sheet. Additional instructions are noted below.)

Special Topics (Student’s Choice)
In addition to the required seven categories of assessment listed above, students will develop an eighth information sheet or pamphlet that includes information targeted either at a specific population or about a specific issue related to psychological assessment not covered in one of the previous sections. Students may choose from one of the following categories:

  • Testing Preschool-Aged Children
  • Testing Elementary School-Aged Children
  • Testing Adolescents
  • Testing Geriatric Patients
  • Testing First Generation Immigrants
  • Testing in Rural Communities
  • Testing English Language Learners
  • Testing Individuals Who Are (Select one: Deaf, Blind, Quadriplegic)
  • Testing Individuals Who Are Incarcerated
  • Testing for Competency to Stand Trial
  • Testing in Child Custody Cases

References (Portrait orientation must be used for the page layout of this section.)
Include a separate reference section that is formatted according to APA style as outlined in the Ashford Writing Center (Links to an external site.)Links to an external site.. The reference list must consist entirely of scholarly sources. For the purposes of this assignment, assessment manuals, the course textbook, chapters from graduate-level textbooks, chapters from professional books, and peer-reviewed journal articles may be used as resource material. A minimum of 16 unique scholarly sources including a minimum of 12 peer-reviewed articles published within the last 10 years from the Ashford University Library must be used within the Assessment Guide. The bulleted list of credible professional and/or educational online resources required for each assessment area will not count toward these totals.

Attention Students: The Masters of Arts in Psychology program is utilizing the Pathbrite portfolio tool as a repository for student scholarly work in the form of signature assignments completed within the program. After receiving feedback for this Assessment Guide, please implement any changes recommended by the instructor, go to Pathbrite  (Links to an external site.)Links to an external site.and upload the revised Assessment Guide to the portfolio. (Use the Pathbrite Quick-Start Guide (Links to an external site.)Links to an external site. to create an account if you do not already have one.) The upload of signature assignments will take place after completing each course. Be certain to upload revised signature assignments throughout the program as the portfolio and its contents will be used in other courses and may be used by individual students as a professional resource tool. See the Pathbrite (Links to an external site.)Links to an external site. website for information and further instructions on using this portfolio tool.

The Assessment Guide

  • Must be 18 pages in length (not including title and reference pages) and formatted according to APA style as outlined in the Ashford Writing Center (Links to an external site.)Links to an external site..
  • Must include a separate title page with the following:
    • Title of guide
    • Student’s name
    • Course name and number
    • Instructor’s name
    • Date submitted
  • Must use at least 16 scholarly sources, including a minimum of 12 peer-reviewed articles from the Ashford University Library.
  • Must document all sources in APA style as outlined in the Ashford Writing Center.
  • Must include a separate reference page that is formatted according to APA style as outlined in the Ashford Writing Center.
  • Must incorporate at least three different methods of presenting information (e.g., text, graphics, images, original cartoons).
  •                              NO PLAGIARISM    DUE TOMORROW NO LATER   

What are the main points/concepts of attachment theory as described in the article?

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1991. “WJI. 61, No. 2, 226-244

Copyright 1991 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0022-3514/91/S3.00

Attachment Styles Among \bung Adults: A Test of a Four-Category Model

Kim Bartholomew Simon Eraser University

Burnaby, British Columbia, Canada

Leonard M. Horowitz Stanford University

A new 4-group model of attachment styles in adulthood is proposed. Four prototypic attachment patterns are defined using combinations of a person’s self-image (positive or negative) and image of others (positive or negative). In Study 1, an interview was developed to yield continuous and categori- cal ratings of the 4 attachment styles. Intercorrelations of the attachment ratings were consistent with the proposed model. Attachment ratings were validated by self-report measures of self-con- cept and interpersonal functioning. Each style was associated with a distinct profile of interper- sonal problems, according to both self- and friend-reports. In Study 2, attachment styles within the family of origin and with peers were assessed independently. Results of Study I were replicated. The proposed model was shown to be applicable to representations of family relations; Ss’ attachment styles with peers were correlated with family attachment ratings.

This article describes a new model of attachment styles in adulthood. Drawing on the theory of Bowlby (1973, 1980, 1982a), two types of internal working models are postulated— an internal model of the self and an internal model of others. Each internal model can be dichotomized as positive or nega- tive to yield four theoretical attachment styles. This article sum- marizes the relevant childhood attachment literature, reviews recent work on adult attachment, describes the new model, and then presents two empirical studies designed to validate the proposed model.

Chi ldhood Attachment and Internal Models

Attachment theory conceptualizes “the propensity of human beings to make strong affectional bonds to particular others” (Bowlby, 1977, p. 201). Bowlby hypothesizes that an attachment system evolved to maintain proximity between infants and their caretakers under conditions of danger or threat. More recent formulations view the attachment system as functioning continuously to provide children with a sense of “felt security” which facilitates exploration by the child (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978; Sroufe & Waters, 1977). The quality of early attachment relationships is thus rooted in the degree to which the infant has come to rely on the attachment figure as a source of security (Ainsworth et al., 1978).

On the basis of infants’ responses to separation from and

This article is based on Kim Bartholomew’s doctoral dissertation at Stanford University

Preparation of this article was supported by Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada doctoral and postdoctoral fellowships to Kim Bartholomew.

We thank James Fuendeling for helping to develop and conduct in- terviews and Diana Doumas, Christine Duffy, Mike Emerzian, Gail Halloway, Stephanie Kondik, and Suzanne Little for coding inter- views.

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Kim Bartholomew, Department of Psychology, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, British Columbia, Canada V5A 1S6.

reunion with caretakers in a structured laboratory procedure, Ainsworth identified three distinct patterns of infant attach- ment: secure, anxious-resistant, and avoidant. Children classi- fied as securely attached welcome their caretaker’s return after a separation and, if distressed, seek proximity and are readily comforted. Infants classified as anxious-resistant show ambiva- lent behavior toward caregivers and an inability to be com- forted on reunion. Infants classified as avoidant avoid proxim- ity or interaction with the caretaker on reunion. Continuity in infant attachment patterns seems to be mediated largely by continuity in the quality of primary attachment relationships (see Lamb, Thompson, Gardner, Charnov, & Estes, 1985).

According to Bowlby’s theory, children, over time, internalize experiences with caretakers in such a way that early attachment relations come to form a prototype for later relationships out- side the family. Bowlby (1973) identifies two key features of these internal representations or working models of attachment: “(a) whether or not the attachment figure is judged to be the sort of person who in general responds to calls for support and protection; [and] (b) whether or not the self is judged to be the sort of person towards whom\anyone, and the attachment fig- ure in particular, is likely to respond in a helpful way” (p. 204). The first concerns the child’s image of other people; the second concerns the child’s image of the self. Recent research has exam- ined the nature of internal working models in relation to chil- dren’s earlier attachment styles. The data show, for example, that children classified as ambivalent hold negative views of themselves, but the data are not as consistent with respect to children classified as avoidant (Cassidy, 1988; Kaplan & Main, 1985; Main, Kaplan, & Cassidy, 1985). A considerable body of research also links the child’s attachment style at 12 or 18 months to the child’s social and emotional adjustment through early childhood (see Bretherton, 1985).

Attachment in Adulthood

A basic principle of attachment theory is that attachment relationships continue to be important throughout the life span (Ainsworth, 1982,1989; Bowlby, 1977,1980,1982b). Although

226

 

 

ATTACHMENT STYLES 227

evidence exists documenting the continuity of attachment-re- lated behaviors (see Belsky & Pensky, 1988; Bowlby, 1973,1980; Ricks, 1985; Rutter, 1988), investigators have only recently ex- amined the relationship between working models of attach- ment and social and emotional adaptation in adults. Main has developed an Adult Attachment Interview (George, Kaplan, & Main, 1987; Main & Goldwyn, 1988) that explores adults* repre- sentations of childhood attachment relations. On the basis of these interviews, mothers have been classified into attachment groups that parallel the three childhood attachment patterns described above and are predictive of the quality of the mother’s interaction with her own child and the security of the child’s attachment (Crowell & Feldman, 1987; Grossmann, Fremmer- Bombik, Rudolf, & Grossmann, 1988; Main et al, 1985). Ko- bak and Sceery (1988) used this procedure to examine young adults’ self- and other-representations, providing some evidence that secure subjects view themselves as relatively undistressed and others as supportive, that dismissive (avoidant) subjects view the self as undistressed and others as unsupportive, and that preoccupied subjects (corresponding to anxious-resistant children) view the self as distressed and others as supportive. (They did not discuss a category of people who might exhibit a negative view of both the self and others.)

Hazan and Shaver (1987) conceptualized romantic love as an attachment process and developed a self-report procedure to classify adults into three categories that correspond to the three attachment styles of childhood. In contrast to Main’s proce- dure, these investigators relied on respondents’ self-reports rather than on inferences from a semi-structured interview. Their results showed that compared with the secure group, the two insecure groups reported more negative experiences and beliefs about love, had a history of shorter romantic relation- ships, and provided less favorable descriptions of their child- hood relationships with parents (see also Collins & Read, 1990). Subjects in the two insecure groups also reported more self- doubt and less acceptability to others than did those endorsing a secure self-description (see also Feeney & Noller, 1990).

These two approaches differed both in the particular attach- ment relationships focused on (parent-child versus love rela- tionships) and in the methodology used for classifying subjects (interview versus self-report). Whereas the interview method identified avoidant adults as people who denied experiencing subjective distress and downplayed the importance of attach- ment needs, the self-report method identified people who re- ported feeling subjective distress and discomfort when they be- come close to others. Thus, a single avoidant-detached category may obscure conceptually separable patterns of avoidance in adulthood. Moreover, although Bowlby (1973) suggested that working models differ in terms of images of self and others, no study has considered all four categories that are logically de- rived by combining the two levels of self-image (positive vs. negative) with the two levels of image of others (positive vs. negative). The present research examined all four of these cate- gories and assessed subjects through an interview as well as through subjects’ own self-reports.

A Model of Adult Attachment

The model of the self and the model of the other as conceptu- alized by Bowlby can be combined to describe prototy pic forms

of adult attachment (Bartholomew, 1990). If a person’s abstract image of the self is dichotomized as positive or negative (the self as worthy of love and support or not) and if the person’s ab- stracted image of the other is also dichotomized as positive or negative (other people are seen as trustworthy and available vs. unreliable and rejecting), then four combinations can be con- ceptualized. Figure 1 shows the four attachment patterns that are derived from a combination of the two dimensions. Each cell represents a theoretical ideal, or prototype (Cantor, Smith, French, & Mezzich, 1980; Horowitz, Wright, Lowenstein, & Parad, 1981; Rosen, 1978), that different people might approxi- mate to different degrees.

Cell I indicates a sense of worthiness (lovability) plus an ex- pectation that other people are generally accepting and respon- sive. Because this cell corresponds conceptually to categories that investigators call securely attached (e.g., Hazan & Shaver, 1987; Main et al, 1985), we labeled it secure. Cell II indicates a sense of unworthiness (unlovability) combined with a positive evaluation of others. This combination of characteristics would lead the person to strive for self-acceptance by gaining the ac- ceptance of valued others. This pattern corresponds concep- tually to Hazan and Shaver’s ambivalent group (Hazan & Shaver, 1987) and to Main’s enmeshed or preoccupied with at- tachment pattern (Main et al., 1985) and is referred to as preoc- cupied. Cell III indicates a sense of unworthiness (unlovability) combined with an expectation that others will be negatively disposed (untrustworthy and rejecting). By avoiding close in- volvement with others, this style enables people to protect themselves against anticipated rejection by others. Although not explicitly discussed in previous work in adult attachment, this style may correspond in part to the avoidant style described by Hazan and Shaver (1987). We therefore labeled it fearful- avoidant. Finally, Cell IV indicates a sense of love-worthiness combined with a negative disposition toward other people. Such people protect themselves against disappointment by avoiding close relationships and maintaining a sense of inde- pendence and invulnerability. This style corresponds concep- tually to the detached or dismissing of attachment attitude de- scribed by Main et al. (1985), so we labeled it dismissive- avoidant.

The dimensions in Figure 1 can also be conceptualized in

MODEL OF SELF (Dependence)

Positive (Low)

Negative (High)

Positive (Low)

MODELOF OTHER (Avoidance)

Negative (High)

CELL I

SECURE Comfortable with

intimacy and autonomy

CELL IV

DISMISSING Dismissing of intimacy

Counter-dependent

CELL II

PREOCCUPIED Preoccupied with

relationships

CELL HI

FEARFUL Fearful of intimacy Socially avoidanl

Figure 1. Model of adult attachment.

 

 

228 KIM BARTHOLOMEW AND LEONARD M. HOROWITZ

terms of dependency on the horizontal axis and the avoidance of intimacy on the vertical axis (see labels in parentheses). De- pendency can vary from low (a positive self-regard is estab- lished internally and does not require external validation) to high (positive self-regard can only be maintained by others’ on- going acceptance). Avoidance of intimacy reflects the degree to which people avoid close contact with others as a result of their expectations of aversive consequences. The dismissing and fearful styles are alike in that both reflect the avoidance of intimacy; they differ, however, in the person’s need for others’ acceptance to maintain a positive self-regard. Similarly, the preoccupied and fearful groups are alike in that both exhibit strong dependency on others to maintain a positive self-regard, but they differ in their readiness to become involved in close relationships. Whereas the preoccupied cell implies a reaching out to others in an attempt to fulfill dependency needs, the fearful cell implies an avoidance of closeness to minimize even- tual disappointment. Therefore, cells in adjoining quadrants of Figure 1 are more similar conceptually than those in opposite quadrants.

Study 1

We administered a semi-structured interview asking subjects to describe their friendship patterns; the subjects’ responses were used to assess the degree to which each person approxi- mated each of the four styles in Figure 1. We also obtained self-report and friends’ ratings of each attachment style, so the three sources of data could be compared. Additional question- naires were administered to subjects and their friends to test hypotheses implied by the model. In particular, self-reports of self-concept, sociability, and interpersonal problems were ob- tained, and self-reports of interpersonal problems were corrob- orated by the judgments of close friends.

The study tested three specific hypotheses. First, multidi- mensional scalings of each set of ratings (by raters, subjects, and friends) were expected to reproduce the organization of Figure 1 across all three sources of data (interview, self-report, and friend-report). Second, self-concept measures were expected to differentiate groups with a positive model of the self (secure and dismissing) from those with a negative model of the self (preoc- cupied and fearful), whereas a sociability measure was expected to differentiate groups with a positive model of others (secure and preoccupied) from those with a negative model of others (fearful and dismissing). Third, the groups were expected to differ from each other in their interpersonal problems. The groups with a negative image of self (preoccupied and fearful) were expected to exhibit problems with passivity and unasserti- veness, whereas those with a negative image of others (fearful and dismissing) were expected to describe problems with socia- lizing and intimacy Problems described by the secure group were not expected to be distinctive in content,

Method

Subjects

Forty female and 37 male students from an introductory psychology class constituted the target sample. They ranged in age from 18 to 22 (M = 19.6); 67% were White, 16% Asian, 5% Hispanic, 8% Black, and

4% other. An equal number of same-sex friends constituted the friend sample. The friends’ age ranged from 18 to 23 (M = 19.8); 65% were White, 13% Asian, 13% Hispanic, 4% Black, and 5% other.

Procedure

Equal numbers of men and women were randomly selected from the subject pool and contacted by telephone. First-year undergraduate stu- dents were excluded to ensure that subjects had been at college long enough to make close friends. Potential subjects were invited to partici- pate with a friend in “a study of friendship and how well people know each other.” Friends were required to be close, same-sex, nonromantic friends whom subjects had known for at least 6 months. Subjects re- ceived course credit for participation, and their accompanying friends were paid $5.

Subjects were tested in groups of two to four friendship pairs. Sub- jects and their friends completed two sets of questionnaires, one re- questing demographic and personality information about themselves and the other asking the same questions of their partner. Instructions were given for participants to answer the second set of questions “ac- cording to your perceptions and knowledge of your friend’s character, feelings or behavior, and not according to how you think your friend may be likely to answer them.” Then each subject and friend were separated to complete the questionnaires. To avoid contrast effects, target subjects completed the self-report questionnaires first, and their friend completed the friend-report first. A second session was then scheduled for subjects to receive the interview.

Measures

Attachment interview. The first author administered a semi-struc- tured interview, which lasted approximately 60 min, to subjects in the target sample. Each interview was tape recorded. The interviewer asked subjects to describe their friendships, romantic relationships, and feelings about the importance of close relationships. If subjects had not been involved in a romantic relationship, they were asked the rea- sons. They were asked about loneliness, shyness, their degreeof trust of others, their impressions of other people’s evaluations of themselves, and their hopes for any changes in their social lives.

On the basis of the interview audio recordings, three raters indepen- dently rated each subject on four 9-point scales describing the subject’s degree of correspondence with each of the four prototypes. The raters comprised two advanced female undergraduate psychology majors and one female graduate student; they were blind to all other measures in the study. A set of criteria (available from the first author) described each prototype, and the rater was instructed to judge how well a sub- ject’s responses matched each of the prototypic descriptions. The four prototypes can be briefly summarized as follows. The secure prototype is characterized by a valuing of intimate friendships, the capacity to maintain close relationships without losing personal autonomy, and a coherence and thoughtfulness in discussing relationships and related issues. The dismissing prototype is characterized by a downplaying of the importance of close relationships, restricted emotionality, an em- phasis on independence and self-reliance, and a lack of clarity or credi- bility in discussing relationships. The preoccupiedprototype is charac- terized by an overinvolvement in close relationships, a dependence on other people’s acceptance fora sense of personal well-being, a tendency to idealize other people, and incoherence and exaggerated emotiona- lity in discussing relationships. The fearful prototype is characterized by an avoidance of close relationships because of a fear of rejection, a sense of personal insecurity, and a distrust of others. Alpha coefficients were computed to assess the reliability of the prototype ratings. The reliabilities ranged from .87 to .95. The ratings were averaged, and the highest of the four average ratings was considered to be the best-fitting

 

 

ATTACHMENT STYLES 229

category for that subject. From this procedure, 47% of the sample was classified as secure, 18% as dismissing, 14% as preoccupied, and 21 % as fearful. In addition, the raters were asked to rate each interview along 15 dimensions of relevance to adult attachment (see Appendix A for definitions of the dimensions).

Self- and friend-reports. All subjects completed demographics and friendship questionnaires, two self-concept measures, and a sociability measure. In addition, the subjects completed the Relationship Ques- tionnaire and the Inventory of Interpersonal Problems twice, once to describe themselves and once to describe their friend. The friend’s version of these questionnaires was identical to the standard self-re- port version, with the exception of wording: Instead of “I try to please other people too much,” for example, the friend’s version read “F[your friend ] tries to please other people too much.” The following question- naires were used:

1. The Demographics Questionnaire included family information (e.g., marital status of parents, number of siblings) and personal activi- ties (e.g., exercise, religious observance). Seven-point items assessed the degree of experienced depression, anxiety, and happiness (with re- versed scoring). These three items were combined into a composite measure of subjective distress ifx = .68).

2. The Friendship Questionnaire contained equivalent demographic, factual, and personal questions about the friend. One item assessed the duration of the friendship, and five items assessed the nature of the friendship (e.g., “Compared with close friendships you’ve had in the past, how close is your friendship with FT*). The latter five items were combined into a friendship closeness scale (target sample a = .80; friend sample a = .86).

3. The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Inventory (Rosenberg, 1965) is a 10- item scale that measures global self-esteem. A sample item is “I cer- tainly feel useless at times” (reverse scored; coefficient a = .85).

4. The Fey Self-Acceptance Scale(Fey, 1955) is a 20-item measure of self-acceptance. A representative item is “I’m pretty satisfied with the way I am” (coefficient a = .86).

5. The Sociability Scale (Cheek & Buss, 1981) is a 5-item measure that assesses the degree to which people like to socialize with others. A sample item is “I like to be with people” (self-report coefficient a = .74; friend-report coefficient = .78).

6. The Relationship Questionnaire is an adaptation of the attach- ment measure developed by Hazan and Shaver (1987). This measure consists of four short paragraphs describing the four attachment styles (see Appendix B). Each respondent is asked to make ratings on a 7- point scale of the degree to which they (or their friend) resemble each of the four styles. These ratings are referred to as the self-report and friend-report attachment ratings.

How the attitude and feelings of students toward their teachers affect their performance.

Submit: Annotation of a Quantitative Research Article

This week, you will submit the annotation of a quantitative research article on a topic of your interest.

An annotation consists of three separate paragraphs that cover three respective components: summary, analysis, and application. These three components convey the relevance and value of the source. As such, an annotation demonstrates your critical thinking about, and authority on, the source. This week’s annotation is a precursor to the annotated bibliography assignment due in Week 10.

An annotated bibliography is a document containing selected sources accompanied by a respective annotation of each source. In preparation for your own future research, an annotated bibliography provides a background for understanding a portion of the existing literature on a particular topic. It is also a useful first step in gathering sources in preparation for writing a subsequent literature review as part of a dissertation.

Please review the assignment instructions below and click on the underlined works for information about how to craft each component of an annotation.

It is recommended that you use the grading rubric as a self-evaluation tool before submitting your assignment.

By Day 7

· Annotate one quantitative research article from a peer-reviewed journal on a topic of your interest.

o Provide the reference list entry for this article in APA Style followed by a three-paragraph annotation that includes:

§ A summary

§ An analysis

§ An application as illustrated in this example

o Format your annotation in Times New Roman, 12-point font, double-spaced. A separate References list page is not needed for this assignment.

Submit your annotation.

An example of a annotation of a quantitative research article;

Quantitative Annotated Bibliography

In light of the intensification of racial profiling conflicts reported by the media in recent years in the United States between African Americans and police officers, it seems the problems between the two groups is escalating. One of the features of racial profiling is rooted in a cultural phase called “Driving While Black.” In the African American and other minority communities, this phrase signifies a perception of the amplified probabilities of being stopped, searched, and detained solely because of the color of your skin. Unlike white motorist, nearly all minority motorist, particularly black motorist have complained of racial profiling deriving from negative racial bias shown by police officers during routine traffic stops. Therefore, the social problem of racial profiling is a topic deserving of researching and exploring to bring about positive social change in society.

Annotated Bibliography

Ridgeway, G. (2006). Assessing the effect of race bias in post-traffic outcomes using prepensity scores. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 22(1), 1-29. doi:10.1007/s10940-005-9000-9

The researcher in this study examined the methods for detecting race bias in the actions of police officers in Oakland, California after a traffic stop. Ridgeway (researcher) utilized the propensity score approach employed successfully by criminological researchers examining domestic violence and analyzing street violence intervention. The propensity score technique afforded Ridgeway an opportunity to evaluate racial biases in police officers while simultaneously comparing the number of traffic stops among the target group (black drivers), against a comparison group (white drivers). Ridgeway’s decision to weigh officer’s discretion to stop, to cite or warn, to search, and to detain were all influenced by the driver’s race. Also, 75 percent of searches conducted by Oakland police were of black drivers. The study suggested a probable cause to stop, cite or warn, search, and detain is more likely among black drivers than non-black drivers. Additionally, the outcome search comparison between black and white drivers is higher among blacks in spite of Oakland officer’s rationale for patrolling high crime in minority neighborhoods. Although race biases in individual officers is hard to correct, the study offered a recommendation that officers stop, cite or warn, search, and detain black & non-blacks equitably in their discretion to stop drivers.

The principal strength of this research design is the utilization and alignment of historical research appropriating the propensity score methodology in their endeavors to investigate racial biases in other domains. Another strength of this examinations is the location of the study. The city of Oakland is among many similar cities across the United States where a bounty of racial profiling incidents is reported, and a context where the bulk of the city’s population is racially diverse. Moreover, a diverse population furnishes researchers with a greater opportunity for avoiding threats to internal validity during the selection process. Despite these strengths, the examinations additionally offered some weaknesses. Take, for example, in the chorus of the observations the researcher should have invested his energies on correlations of gender and racial profiling to detect if racial biases exist. Additionally, the data suggested stronger presents of racial profiling against blacks than whites during post stop encounters with police.  However, the research lacks any data regarding racial profiling experienced in Asian and Hispanic communities.  To further boost the strength of the study, the researcher could have employed their methods across other populations. Perhaps applying their techniques or methods across the Muslim community, where traffic stops have risen may also point to racial profiling by police. Therefore, by observing various groups, in different settings, and employing similar application to each situation, researchers then can better grasp the causes of racial profiling in today’s society.

This examination is not only value added to criminology, but also to the social science community as well. Ridgeway’s research can be delivered across multiple policing training initiatives designed to overcome and bring awareness to racial profiling during routine traffic stops. Additionally, this research is beneficial to changing negative policing practices employed only in minority neighborhoods.  Ridgeway’s enthusiasm to study the growing problem of racial profiling will aid on the road to implementing new policy changes and legislation regarding this issue in the future, though he never implies this in his study. Finally, I would like to see this data shared among all stakeholders, police and victims (minorities) to improve on building positive relationships in their communities.

Research Topic

How the attitude and feelings of students toward their teachers affect their performance.

Positive attitudes and feeling of students toward their teachers contribute a lot to their performance since it helps them to develop positive attitudes toward various subjects. Additionally, it promotes the creation of an excellent learning environment.

My worldview is supported by the existentialism and which is a philosophical orientation, and it states that students should be permitted to ask questions, perform their inquiries and make their conclusion of various issues and this is possible if students have positive attitudes toward their teachers (Babbie, 2017). Additionally, students should be granted freedom of choice since different students have different ability and potentials. They should be given an opportunity to choose the field in which they fit correctly.

Based on ontology, it is known that different students are talented differently and have different abilities, therefore, a good and stable relationship between teachers and student, and which is created if a student develops positive attitudes toward their teachers can facilitate teachers to known and understand abilities of various students and thus guide them to achieve their dream (Burkholder et al., 2016). Epistemologically, we can research to find out how the attitude and feeling of students toward their teachers affect their performance.

There are various research approaches which can be used in this study. A survey is one of the best since it will involve selection of students from different schools in the region and ask them how attitudes and feeling of students towards their teachers affect their performance and as a result will be able to know whether attitudes and perceptions of students towards there teachers change their performance.

References

Babbie, E. (2017). Basics of social research (7th ed.). Boston, MA: Cengage Learning.

Barrett, E., & Bolt, B. (Eds.). (2014). Practice as research: Approaches to creative arts inquiry. Ib Tauris.

Burkholder, G. J., Cox, K. A., & Crawford, L. M. (2016). The scholar-practitioners guide to research design. Baltimore, MD: Laureate Publishing.