Issues Of Advocacy And Social Justice

Prior to beginning work on this discussion read Hill (2013) “Partnering with a Purpose: Psychologists as Advocates in Organizations,” Cohen, Lee, & McIlwraith (2012) “The Psychology of Advocacy and the Advocacy of Psychology,” Heinowitz, et al. (2012) “Identifying Perceived Personal Barriers to Public Policy Advocacy within Psychology,” Lewis, Ratts, Paladino, & Toporek (2011) “Social Justice Counseling and Advocacy: Developing New Leadership Roles and Competencies,” and Fox (2008) “Advocacy: The Key to the Survival and Growth of Professional Psychology” articles.

For this discussion, you will compare the various professional activities common to clinical and counseling psychologists and assume the role of an advocate for a client in one of the case studies from Case Studies in Abnormal Psychology (Gorenstein & Comer, 2015). Select a case study that has not been covered in this course or in the PSY645 course, and identify systemic barriers, sociopolitical factors, and multicultural issues impacting the client at the micro, meso, exo, and/or macro levels. Develop an action plan that outlines how you might advocate for the client at each appropriate level of the ecological model. Identify two potential partnerships that you would establish in order to support your client and those like him or her outside of the therapeutic environment.

Identifying Perceived Personal Barriers to Public Policy Advocacy Within Psychology

Amy E. Heinowitz, Kelly R. Brown, Leah C. Langsam, Steven J. Arcidiacono, Paige L. Baker, Nadimeh H. Badaan, Nancy I. Zlatkin, and Ralph E. (Gene) Cash

Nova Southeastern University

Public policy advocacy within the profession of psychology appears to be limited and in its infancy. Various hypothesized barriers to advocacy within the field are analyzed in this study. Findings indicate that those who advocate do so regardless of whether the issue is specific to the profession of psychology or specific to another field. Furthermore, several components, including disinterest, uncertainty, and unawareness, were identified as barriers to advocacy. However, all barriers were subsumed by a lack of awareness of public policy issues. By identifying barriers to advocacy in psychology, programs promot- ing advocacy could be fine-tuned to address the lack of knowledge, which inhibits students, profession- als, and clinicians from engaging in the essential role of public policy advocacy.

Keywords: advocacy, public policy, professional involvement

Supplemental materials: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0029161.supp

There is an urgent and growing need for professional and social justice advocacy within the psychological community (Ratts & Hutchins, 2009; Kiselica & Robinson, 2001; Ratts, D’Andrea, & Arredondo, 2004; Toporek, Gerstein, Fouad, Roysircar, & Israel, 2006). Psychology, as a field as well as a profession, aims to reduce negative treatment outcomes and to enhance personal well- being through research and practice (Council of Specialties in Professional Psychology, 2009; American Psychological Associa- tion, 2010b). The viability of the profession and its capacity to provide fundamental and essential services are directly affected by legislation and regulations (Barnett, 2004). As a result, advocacy is integral to the roles of all psychologists, with the future and success of their profession and careers depending on their incor- poration of advocacy into their professional identity (Burney et al.,

2009). Despite the recognition and high appraisal of advocacy, little information is known about how, why, and to what degree individual professionals within the psychological arena participate in public policy advocacy.

The essential question is what does the advocacy role entail? That is the first concern that negatively influences advocacy rates—the vague, ill-defined, and at best multifaceted definition applied to this concept (Trusty & Brown, 2005). It is likely that the act of advocating is conceptualized in markedly distinct ways from one practitioner to the next and, in some cases, may even be inaccurate (Lating, Barnett, & Horowitz, 2009). Lating et al. (2009) describe advocacy as “a process of informing and assisting decision makers, [which] entails developing active ‘citizen psy- chologists’ who promote the interest of clients, health care sys-

This article was published Online First July 2, 2012. AMY E. HEINOWITZ is currently a fourth year PhD student at Nova Southeast- ern University. She previously received her Master of Arts in Psychology from Adelphi University. Her areas of professional interest are in developmental psychology, attachment theory, contextual approaches to trauma resolution, substance use, and professional issues in advocacy work. KELLY R. BROWN is currently a fourth year PhD student at Nova Southeastern University, where she previously received her Master of Science in Clinical Psychology. Her areas of professional interest include advocacy advancement and stigma reduction, child and family psychology, crisis intervention, peer victimization and youth violence, and suicide prevention. LEAH C. LANGSAM is a fifth year PsyD student at Nova Southeastern University, where she also received her Master of Science in Clinical Psychology. Her areas of professional interest are in child and adolescent trauma, the assessment of psychopathology in youth, and professional issues in advocacy work. STEVEN J. ARCIDIACONO is currently a fourth year PhD student at Nova Southeastern University where he also received his en route Master of Science in Psychology. His primary areas of research and practice include youth physical fitness, behavioral issues in adolescents, research method- ology, and advocacy in psychology.

PAIGE L. BAKER is currently a second year PsyD student at Nova South- eastern University. She previously received a Bachelor of Arts in Psychol- ogy and in Women & Gender Studies from Georgetown University. Her areas of professional interest include multicultural and diversity issues, military psychology, and professional issues in advocacy work. NADIMEH H. BADAAN is currently a third year PsyD student at Nova Southeastern University. She obtained her Masters of Arts in Forensic Psychology from John Jay College of Criminal Justice. Her professional interests are in forensic psychology, battered women syndrome, posttrau- matic stress, child sexual abuse, and the psychology of advocacy. NANCY I. ZLATKIN is a fifth year PsyD student at Nova Southeastern University. She holds her Master of Science degree from Nova Southeast- ern University as well. Her professional interests include substance abuse, bullying, solution focused therapies, telehealth, and professional advocacy. RALPH E. (GENE) CASH received his PhD in School Psychology from New York University. He is an associate professor and director of the School Psychology Assessment and Consultation clinic at Nova Southeastern University. His areas of research and practice include suicide prevention, the psychology of public advocacy, and school psychology. CORRESPONDENCE CONCERNING THIS ARTICLE should be addressed to Amy E. Heinowitz, Center for Psychological Studies, Nova Southeastern University, 3301 College Avenue, Fort Lauderdale, FL 33317. E-mail: ah916@nova.edu

Professional Psychology: Research and Practice © 2012 American Psychological Association 2012, Vol. 43, No. 4, 372–378 0735-7028/12/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0029161

372

T hi

s do

cu m

en t i

s co

py ri

gh te

d by

th e

A m

er ic

an P

sy ch

ol og

ic al

A ss

oc ia

tio n

or o

ne o

f i ts

a lli

ed p

ub lis

he rs

. T

hi s

ar tic

le is

in te

nd ed

s ol

el y

fo r t

he p

er so

na l u

se o

f t he

in di

vi du

al u

se r a

nd is

n ot

to b

e di

ss em

in at

ed b

ro ad

ly .

 

 

tems, public health and welfare issues, and professional psychol- ogy” (p. 201). Trusty and Brown (2005) offer a streamlined summary of the various descriptions of advocacy as “identifying unmet needs and taking actions to change the circumstances that contribute to the problem or inequity” (p. 259). Regardless of definition, advocacy remains a necessary component of the psy- chology profession (Burney et al., 2009; Fox, 2008).

Advocacy can be divided into three sectors: public policy, social justice, and professional advocacy (see Figure 1). Public policy advocacy is defined as the attempt to influence practice, policy and legislation through education, lobbying and communication with legislators and elected officials. Social justice advocacy, most broadly, involves championing for the basic human and civil rights of all people regardless of race, class, gender, or socioeconomic status. In the context of psychology, however, social justice advo- cacy can more aptly be understood as the recognition “that fairness and justice entitle all persons to access to and benefit from the contributions of psychology and to equal quality in the processes, procedures, and services being conducted by psychologists” (American Psychological Association Code of Ethics, 2010a). Lastly, professional advocacy is a synthesis of both public policy and social justice advocacy. Professional advocacy in the field of professional psychology demands that clinicians advocate not only for fair access to appropriate services but also for the important legislative changes necessary to enhance the quality of life of patients and at-risk populations.

The literature cites several important triumphs within the field (e.g., mental health parity) that can be attributed to the efforts of diligent advocates. Perhaps one of the greatest events was the combined advocacy effort of individual psychologists working with the National Association for the Advancement of Colored People (NAACP) in response to the Brown v. Board of Education Supreme Court case in 1954 (Benjamin & Crouse, 2002). Aware- ness of these accomplishments is important to understanding psy- chology’s roots in public and social advocacy and to provide

impetus for continuing advocacy efforts. However, it should be noted that a great deal more work is still necessary (DeLeon, Loftis, Ball, & Sullivan, 2006; Fox, 2008). Expanding and pro- tecting markets, maintaining funding, providing education and training, and disseminating important information to the public are just a few current initiatives requiring ongoing advocacy (Fox, 2008). Fox (2008) advised, “addressing such an agenda will re- quire efforts far beyond the scope and magnitude of all our past efforts put together” (p. 634).

Despite the acknowledgment of advocacy as an essential re- sponsibility for psychologists, many individuals remain unin- formed and uninvolved. With regard to financial support, psychol- ogists rank among the lowest contributors when compared with other medical professions (Pfeiffer, 2007). Furthermore, psychol- ogists have maintained poor political representation at the national level (DeLeon et al., 2006). Of utmost concern resulting from this lack of involvement is the forfeiture of opportunities to provide input on critical issues. This, in turn, would affect the overall future of the profession as well as the future careers of individual psychologists and the well-being of clients.

Previous research has identified a number of potential barriers to public policy advocacy, which reinforces the immediate need for further research, not only to identify obstacles, but also to pave pathways of enhanced efforts. Myers and Sweeney (2004) initially introduced an exploration of obstacles to professional advocacy via a survey of 71 professionals in the counseling community in local, regional, or national leadership positions. Fifty-eight percent of respondents cited inadequate resources as their primary obstacle to advocacy. Additionally, 51% indicated there was opposition by other providers, 51% noted a lack of collaboration, and 42% suggested a lack of training was responsible for insufficient advo- cacy efforts. While these findings highlight important structural and fiscal challenges, it is prudent to examine the personal barriers, which may further hinder psychologists’ participation in advocacy.

Individual experiences and personality traits may impede psy- chologists’ participation in advocacy in significant ways. Previous literature highlights the impact of awareness (Gronholt, 2009) and professional agendas (Lating et al., 2009) on psychologists’ par- ticipation in advocacy endeavors. More specifically, Gronholt (2009) revealed that despite active participation in academia, stu- dents and faculty cited an absence of interest in advocacy and inadequate awareness of advocacy issues and opportunities as the most significant factors inhibiting participation. These findings suggest that a lack of training or education is a considerable and consistent obstacle in advocacy participation.

When assessing the impact of awareness and training upon psychologists’ underrepresentation in the advocate role, it is nec- essary to evaluate the perceived personal sacrifices associated with some advocacy efforts. According to Chang, Hays, and Milliken (2009) there are numerous perceived personal costs. For example, they cite burnout, job loss, and harassment from other profession- als who may have the belief that client difficulties are not system- ically related. Additionally, psychologists are likely to contextu- alize their chosen advocacy issues as either inappropriate or incongruent with their professional agenda (Chang et al., 2009; Lating et al., 2009). Similarly Benjamin and Course (2002) suggest “psychologists’ aversions to political or social pronouncements have a long history in American psychology, grounded in part in the belief that science and application are separate activities and in

Professional advocacy

Public policy

advocacy

Social jus�ce

advocacy

Figure 1. Three facets of advocacy roles for professional psychologists. Social justice advocacy entails those efforts that are aimed at facilitating the fair, beneficent, and just treatment of all individuals. Public policy advocacy addresses the more legislative and governmental efforts. Lastly, professional advocacy encompasses both social and public policy advo- cacy.

373PERCEIVED BARRIERS TO PUBLIC POLICY ADVOCACY

T hi

s do

cu m

en t i

s co

py ri

gh te

d by

th e

A m

er ic

an P

sy ch

ol og

ic al

A ss

oc ia

tio n

or o

ne o

f i ts

a lli

ed p

ub lis

he rs

. T

hi s

ar tic

le is

in te

nd ed

s ol

el y

fo r t

he p

er so

na l u

se o

f t he

in di

vi du

al u

se r a

nd is

n ot

to b

e di

ss em

in at

ed b

ro ad

ly .

 

 

the long-standing prejudices held against applied work” (p. 46). In other words, some psychologists experience difficulty aligning their professional identities and values with larger, sociopolitical issues and may fear professional ramifications.

In addition to these perceived challenges, advocacy literature must articulate the personal attributes that influence effective in- volvement in public policy advocacy. Interestingly, an identified barrier to psychologists’ participation in advocacy relates to the nature of the person drawn to the profession. Psychologists are likely to focus their attention on the interpersonal issues that affect clients rather than considering the larger, systemic issues contrib- uting to pathology (Chang et al., 2009; Lating et al., 2009). In fact, it may be that psychologists view advocacy on an individual-level rather than global-level. For example, fostering development of self-advocacy skills and encouraging clients to be resourceful may be a primary focus rather than becoming an advocate for the clients or the field (Waldmann & Blackwell, 2010). Perhaps this tendency precludes psychologists from identifying or promoting the need for social change.

Despite the helpful studies previously conducted on advocacy, there are distinct limitations to the current state of advocacy research. The literature related directly to advocacy within psy- chology is underdeveloped. There is an immediate need for re- search assessing perceived barriers to participation in advocacy via the development of “rigorous assessment tools to evaluate practi- tioner awareness, knowledge, and skills related to advocacy coun- seling efforts” (Green, McCollum, & Hays, 2008, p. 26). This study not only moves forward the field of research assessing perceived barriers to psychologists’ involvement in public policy, but it also suggests important implications for guiding enhance- ment of professional advocacy efforts and directing training pro- grams.

Statement of Problem

Advocacy within the profession of psychology appears to be limited and in its infancy. Strikingly, research shows that other fields engage in high rates of advocacy. This study seeks to understand what the perceived barriers are to advocacy within the field of psychology. Further, it strives to elucidate whether there are differences between those who advocate specifically on behalf of psychological issues versus those who may advocate in other related domains.

Method

Participants were recruited via a mass email sent to the graduate psychology department of a private southeastern university. Those who decided to participate completed an anonymous online survey created with the purpose of understanding barriers to advocacy. The survey contained a total of 18 items that included demo- graphic information, rates of advocacy involvement, and attitudes toward various types of advocacy efforts. Items followed a four- choice response scale measuring frequency of behavior (e.g., “I advocate for issues within my specific field of psychology”: very frequently, somewhat frequently, rarely, never), and belief in per- sonal effectiveness (e.g., “I do not believe my participation will generate much of an effect”: very relevant, somewhat relevant, somewhat irrelevant, very irrelevant”). Items were chosen based

off of the literature review, which identified several barriers to advocacy within the field of psychology. The portions of the survey that were used for the current analysis can be found in the online-only data supplement.

Participants ranged in age from 18 to 64 years, with most between the ages of 18 and 34. The majority of participants were students (63.5%), with the remaining sample consisting of alumni, staff, and faculty members. Of those who endorsed being a student affiliate, almost 60% were working toward a postgraduate degree (masters or doctorate).

Pearson correlations, a stepwise linear regression, and a princi- pal components analysis were used to examine the data.

Results

Descriptives

Participants included 85 adults from the previously mentioned university. However, only 59 participants completed demographic information. The sample was predominantly composed of females (94.8%). Participants were asked to select their age via different ranges: 20.3% were between the ages of 18 –24, 44% were be- tween the ages of 25–34, 11.9% were between the ages of 35– 44, 20.3% were between the ages of 45–54, and 3.4% were between the ages of 55– 64. The percentages reported were rounded to the nearest tenth; as such, the valid percent equals 99.9%. The sample consisted predominately of students (91.5%) currently working toward a master’s degree (38.6%) or a doctoral degree (38.6%) in psychology or a closely related field. The remainder of the sample consisted of university faculty (3.4%), alumni (3.4%), and clinical staff (1.7%). The self-described political orientations of partici- pants varied among very liberal (20.7%), somewhat liberal (27.6%), moderate (37.9%), somewhat conservative (12.1%), and very conservative (1.7%).

Pearson Correlations

To investigate the influence of barriers to advocacy within psychology, several statistical analyses were conducted on re- sponses to the online survey. Pearson correlations between self- reported relevance of potential barriers and advocacy in psychol- ogy are presented in Table 1. Results indicated that those who advocate more frequently tend to believe that the relevant barriers are having a poor past experience (r � �.261, p � .048) and not believing one has enough knowledge to discuss issues competently (r � �.348, p � .007). Meanwhile, feeling as though not being aware of current public policy issues was a relative inhibitor to advocacy was significantly correlated with less advocacy (r � .404, p � .001). Additionally, significant correlations were present between several potential barriers, indicating a considerable degree of consistency among items.

Stepwise Linear Regression

Although some barriers to advocacy were individually cor- related with advocacy participation, the overlap of variance among items can make it difficult to determine which barriers are most important in predicting advocacy. Thus, a stepwise linear regression was used to determine which predictors (i.e.,

374 HEINOWITZ ET AL.

T hi

s do

cu m

en t i

s co

py ri

gh te

d by

th e

A m

er ic

an P

sy ch

ol og

ic al

A ss

oc ia

tio n

or o

ne o

f i ts

a lli

ed p

ub lis

he rs

. T

hi s

ar tic

le is

in te

nd ed

s ol

el y

fo r t

he p

er so

na l u

se o

f t he

in di

vi du

al u

se r a

nd is

n ot

to b

e di

ss em

in at

ed b

ro ad

ly .

 

 

barriers) work in combination with one another to predict advocacy involvement within one’s specific field of psychology most effectively. The following nine predictor variables were entered into the model: unawareness of public policy issues, lack of belief in the effect one’s participation will have on issues, lack of time, disinterest, belief that one is not persuasive enough, poor past experiences, lack of awareness of opportu- nities to become involved, belief that there is no need for advocacy, and belief that one does not have enough knowledge to discuss such issues competently.

After conducting a stepwise linear regression analysis, it can be concluded that the overall model significantly predicts public policy advocacy, F(1, 54) � 17.270, p � .001 (A statistical table summarizing the results is available in the online-only data sup- plement). Results of the stepwise linear regression procedure in- dicated that the only significant barrier present, after considering overlap of variance among variables, was awareness of public policy issues (r � .492, R2 � .242).

Principal Components Analysis

To investigate the constructs behind lack of advocacy within psychology, a principal components analysis (PCA) with varimax rotation was conducted. The results of these analyses are available in the online-only data supplement. Using Kaiser’s eigenvalue- greater-than-one-rule, three components were extracted from the 10 barriers. Items loaded onto each component were considered if they had a correlation (i.e., loading) of at least .4 with a given component. Given these criteria, the first component yielded could be named “disinterest,” the second component could be named “uncertainty,” and the third component could be named “unaware- ness.”

The three components accounted for 60% of the total variance after performing a PCA. The first component contributed 28% of the variance, the second component contributed 21%, and the third component contributed 11%. These three factors were reproduced on the Extraction Sums of Squared Loadings, indicating that only these factors had eigenvalues that were greater than or equal to one.

The first component included not having an interest in partici- pating, not believing there is a need for advocacy, not believing that participation will generate an effect, having a poor past expe- rience, and not wanting to give out information (termed “disinter- est”). The second component included not having enough knowl- edge and not feeling persuasive enough (termed “uncertainty”). Finally, the third component included lack of awareness of public advocacy issues as well as opportunities to advocate (termed “unawareness”).

The results of the PCA taken in tandem with the results of the correlation and regression analysis indicate that there are three distinct components regarding barriers to advocacy (disinterest, uncertainty, and unawareness); however, the influence of several barriers (e.g., poor past experience, lack of knowledge) are sub- sumed under the impact of unawareness of public policy issues.

Discussion

Results indicate that those who advocate do so regardless of whether the issue lies within or outside of their specific field. More simply, those who advocate, advocate. This finding may be indic- ative of unique personal characteristics of those who are involved in advocacy efforts. Relative to other health professions, those drawn to professional psychology may be more interested in individual issues rather than larger sociopolitical concerns (Lating et al., 2009). In other words, psychologists may more readily advocate for individuals but advocate less for larger platforms. This advocacy pattern may be further influenced by the tendency for public policy issues to be presented in polarized views, in contrast to the tendency for psychologists to view things in shades of gray.

Results further revealed that several barriers were independently correlated with psychologists’ participation in advocacy; however, a substantial overlap of variance was also indicated. Considering poor past experiences with advocacy as a barrier was, ironically, associated with greater participation in advocacy. This suggests that negative past experiences do not deter people from advocating in the future. It is also likely that those who advocate are more apt

Table 1 Pearson Correlation Matrix Among Barriers to Advocacy Efforts and Self-Reported Public Policy Advocacy

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11

1. No time 1 2. Unaware of opportunities �.205 1 3. Lack of interest .158 �.169 1 4. Belief that there is no need for

advocacy .104 .077 .546�� 1 5. Belief that participation will be

ineffective .078 �.168 .393�� .371�� 1 6. Poor past experiences .153 .039 .331� .423�� .479�� 1 7. I do not want to give out my

information .274� �.097 .365�� .286� .313� .343�� 1 8. Lack of knowledge to discuss issues �.264� .223 �.055 .017 .309� .060 �.008 1 9. Belief that person lacks persuasiveness .065 �.107 .152 .326� .352�� .149 .024 .394�� 1

10. Unaware of current issues �.096 .475�� .065 .252 .053 �.017 �.194 .504�� .404�� 1 11. Advocating for issues within one’s field

of psychology .225 �.176 �.250 �.115 �.044 .261� .201 �.348�� �.234 �.404�� 1

� p � .05. �� p � .01.

375PERCEIVED BARRIERS TO PUBLIC POLICY ADVOCACY

T hi

s do

cu m

en t i

s co

py ri

gh te

d by

th e

A m

er ic

an P

sy ch

ol og

ic al

A ss

oc ia

tio n

or o

ne o

f i ts

a lli

ed p

ub lis

he rs

. T

hi s

ar tic

le is

in te

nd ed

s ol

el y

fo r t

he p

er so

na l u

se o

f t he

in di

vi du

al u

se r a

nd is

n ot

to b

e di

ss em

in at

ed b

ro ad

ly .

 

 

to have negative (as well as potentially positive) experiences than those who do not advocate.

The overall regression model with nine predictor variables entered in was deemed statistically significant. The only significant barrier, however, was awareness of public policy issues. In other words, much of the predictive influence of the assessed barriers to advocacy was actually subsumed under the barrier of feeling unaware of public policy issues for which to advocate. For example, not believing one has enough knowledge to discuss issues competently inhibits public policy advocacy, but not over and above the influence of not being aware of public policy advocacy issues in the first place. These results suggest that lack of awareness of advocacy issues strongly inhibits involvement in psychology advocacy. In fact, the impact of some other speculated barriers might actually be better accounted for by this lack of awareness. For instance, psychologists or psychology students may feel as though they lack adequate knowledge to discuss public policy issues simply because they are in the dark about what the issues are.

Furthermore, areas previously assumed to be relevant barriers to advocacy, (e.g., unawareness of opportunities to become involved, lack of time) appear less important than expected. Instead of emphasizing awareness of avenues for advocacy or suggesting time-efficient opportunities, interventions should be aimed primar- ily at improving education with regard to current, relevant public policy concerns. Lating et al. (2009) indicated that 60% of psy- chology programs do not offer specific advocacy training. How- ever, the authors note that 88% cover advocacy issues in class. This suggests that improvements in education are slowly develop- ing and perhaps will someday result in full-fledged advocacy training as an integral part of psychology programs.

Although lack of awareness was found to be the most meaning- ful barrier, moderate semipartial correlations (i.e., correlations after considering the impact of other investigated barriers) suggest future studies are needed to establish the roles of variables to assess interest in participating in as well as the belief in a need for public policy advocacy. In the current study, these variables failed to meet statistical significance as predictors of advocacy; however, increased sample size in future replications may provide the power necessary to yield a significant result.

After performing a PCA, three components emerged. The three components accounted for 60% of the total variance. The first component contributed 28% of the variance (not having an interest in participating, not believing there is a need for advocacy, not believing that participation will generate an effect, having a poor past experience, and not wanting to give out information). The second component contributed 21% (not having enough knowl- edge and not feeling persuasive enough), and the third component contributed 11% (lack of awareness of public advocacy issues as well as opportunities to advocate).

The three components identified by the PCA (disinterest, un- certainty, and unawareness) as barriers to advocacy corroborate the findings of previous advocacy research (Myers & Sweeney, 2004; Gronholt, 2009). The first component, termed “disinterest,” in- cluded not having an interest in participating, not believing there is a need for advocacy, not believing that participation will generate an effect, having a poor past experience, and not wanting to give out information. Though this is a complex and multifaceted com- ponent, results remain consistent with previous research suggest- ing that advocacy is not a priority among many psychologists due

to a general lack of interest (Myers & Sweeney, 2004). More explicitly, the authors found that 28% of clinicians did not view advocacy as a priority. Furthermore, 27% of clinicians reported that they did not have any interest in advocating (Myers & Sweeney, 2004). Other studies have used the lack of “motivational spark” as a synonym for the disinterest in participating experi- enced by professionals (London, 2010).

The second component, termed “uncertainty,” included items such as not having enough knowledge and not feeling persuasive enough. The lack of knowledge identified by our participants is likely related to a lack of training in advocacy. Myers and Sweeney (2004) established that 41% of their sample found a lack of training to be a significant barrier in advocacy work. When psy- chology programs fail to emphasize advocacy, students are likely to graduate without the confidence and tools necessary to advocate effectively. According to London (2010), a lack of confidence impacts motivation and the manner in which psychologists con- ceptualize problems and the need for change.

Finally, the third component, termed “unawareness,” included lack of awareness of public advocacy issues as well as opportuni- ties to advocate. Again, our results corroborate the findings of Myers and Sweeney (2004) that suggest a lack of awareness of advocacy issues is a significant barrier to participation in advo- cacy.

There are several limitations inherent in the design of the current study. For one, the sample was drawn from one university in the southeastern region of the United States. There may be issues with generalizability to the population of the United States as a whole. Additionally, the small sample size (N � 86) may further reduce applicability to the general population of professional psycholo- gists. As such, the results should be interpreted within the context of existing within an exploratory framework. Further research is needed to examine characteristics in more diverse samples. Fur- thermore, the survey used was exploratory at best. Future studies ought to expand on the current template to include questions with greater variability in responses, as well as to include additional items or perceived barriers.

Participation in advocacy within the profession of psychology is essential because public policy drives professional functioning. The future of the field and of the people served by psychologists depends on advocacy efforts. Consequently, a careful consider- ation of the interaction among the three components identified in our study can provide valuable insight into improving advocacy within psychology. First, advocacy must become a valued asset to the field. As previous research has indicated, nearly half of psy- chologists admit that advocacy is not a priority (Kindsfater, 2008). Before the other barriers to advocacy can be addressed, psychol- ogists need to perceive advocacy as an integral part of their profession. Once advocacy is valued, the lack of preparation and awareness can be addressed through graduate training programs and continuing education courses. Ideally, the increased valuation of advocacy, combined with the necessary tools and avenues to pursue it, will ignite motivation for psychologists to take their roles as advocates seriously.

Implications

Advocacy is a major component of psychology and mental health awareness. Although no significant trait or construct differ-

376 HEINOWITZ ET AL.

T hi

s do

cu m

en t i

s co

py ri

gh te

d by

th e

A m

er ic

an P

sy ch

ol og

ic al

A ss

oc ia

tio n

or o

ne o

f i ts

a lli

ed p

ub lis

he rs

. T

hi s

ar tic

le is

in te

nd ed

s ol

el y

fo r t

he p

er so

na l u

se o

f t he

in di

vi du

al u

se r a

nd is

n ot

to b

e di

ss em

in at

ed b

ro ad

ly .

 

 

ences were found between participants who advocate within or outside their own field, this study did illustrate the essential need for advocacy training. This finding is crucial because it illustrates that lack of motivation or unwillingness to advocate is not primar- ily responsible for preventing advocacy; rather it is a deficiency in understanding or simply being aware of relevant issues. This lack of knowledge implies that the psychological community should seek to enlighten individual members not only about advocacy procedures, how they work, and the vast benefits that can emerge, but also about specific issues. Psychology students and profes- sional practitioners are typically unaware of how much their indi- vidual contributions can actually help. People may not spend time or money advocating if they do not believe any results will emerge from their efforts. Hence, steps should be taken to highlight positive advocacy experiences and successful policy changes.

If professional psychologists actively supported relevant issues regarding mental health, the field of psychology would advance at a faster rate. To initiate this, implementing advocacy education in continuing education classes, mandatory seminars, and yearly con- ferences would compel psychologists to hear the relevant issues at hand. Professional psychologists may be overwhelmed with a heavy workload and not have time to individually research and participate in public policy advocacy. However, when made aware of significant concerns related to mental health, by nature, profes- sional psychologists will be unable to ignore them.

As for spreading the importance of advocacy among profes- sional facilities outside of the psychological field, companies can provide in-house training to employees to increase comfort and familiarity with the advocacy process. Because there are numerous areas in which individuals are interested, education can be pro- vided according to the relevance of each specific institution. Peo- ple in general are more likely to support issues that have meaning to them. Tailoring advocacy education in this manner may not only attract a greater amount of people but may also make the under- standing of advocacy more simplistic.

Furthermore, there is a lack of awareness among society about which issues are most pertinent to be advocated for. It is therefore critical to provide timely information pertaining to relevant public policy issues for which the public can advocate. Creating public advocacy groups can also help disseminate information and in- crease opportunities for positive experiences. Increasing layman’s confidence in advocacy can be accomplished by providing training opportunities via open workshops to create collaborative advocacy endeavors.

The findings presented in this study carry valuable implications for efforts aimed at enhancing participation in advocacy. Lating et al. (2009) suggest that the continued separation of professional and educational agendas in the training of psychologists may contrib- ute to the profession’s deficient involvement in advocacy. Specif- ically, psychology is the only major health profession to maintain an academic training model despite the creation of professional training programs. The lack of advocacy training appears to con- tribute to the development and maintenance of barriers such as lack of awareness of and lack of perceived competence in discuss- ing public policy issues.

Efforts to increase psychologists’ participation in public policy advocacy must begin early on and be integrated throughout their curricula. Pertinent public policy issues fit well into courses on ethics, diversity, assessment, and even intervention. Similarly,

discussion about and training in the advocacy role may be rein- forced through clinical training and supervision. In addition to incorporated teaching lessons, specific coursework in public policy advocacy might aid students in developing skills used to advocate, while increasing comfort, enhancing familiarity, and expanding knowledge of current issues.

References

American Psychological Association. (2010a). Ethical principles of psy- chologists and code of conduct (2002, Amended June 1, 2010). Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/ethics/code/index.aspx

American Psychological Association. (2010b). Public description of clin- ical psychology. Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/Ed./graduate/ specialize/clinical.aspx

Barnett, J. E. (2004). On being a psychologist and how to save our profession. Independent Practitioner, 24, 45– 46.

Benjamin, L. T., Jr., & Crouse, E. M. (2002). The American Psychological Association’s response to Brown v. Board of Education: The case of Kenneth B. Clark. American Psychologist, 57, 38 –50. doi:10.1037/ 0003-066X.57.1.38

Burney, J. P., Celeste, B. L., Johnson, J. D., Klein, N. C., Nordal, K. C., & Portnoy, S. M. (2009). Mentoring professional psychologists: Programs for career development, advocacy, and diversity. Professional Psychol- ogy: Research and Practice, 40, 292–298. doi:10.1037/a0015029

Chang, C. Y., Hays, D. G., & Milliken, T. F. (2009). Addressing social justice issues in supervision: A call for client and professional advocacy. The Clinical Supervisor, 28, 20 –35. doi:10.1080/07325220902855144

Council of Specialties in Professional Psychology. (2009). CoSPP bylaws. Retrieved from http://cospp.org/bylaws

DeLeon, P. H., Loftis, C. W., Ball, V., & Sullivan, M. J. (2006). Navigating politics, policy, and procedure: A firsthand perspective of advocacy on behalf of the profession. Professional Psychology: Research and Prac- tice, 37, 146 –153. doi:10.1037/0735-7028.37.2.146

Fox, R. E. (2008). Advocacy: The key to the survival and growth of professional psychology. Professional Psychology: Research and Prac- tice, 39, 633– 637. doi:10.1037/0735-7028.39.6.633

Green, E. J., McCollum, V. C., & Hays, D. G. (2008). Teaching advocacy counseling within a social justice framework: Implications for school counselors and educators. Journal for Social Action in Counseling and Psychology, 1, 14 –30.

Gronholt, J. M. (2009). An exploration of the differences in psychology faculty and graduate students’ participation in mental health legislation and barriers to advocacy. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B. Sciences and Engineering, 70, 2.

Kindsfater, H. C. (2008). Factors related to psychologists’ participation in professional advocacy (Unpublished dissertation). The University of South Dakota: South Dakota.

Kiselica, M. S., & Robinson, M. (2001). Bringing advocacy counseling to life: The history, issues, and human dramas of social justice work in counseling. Journal of Counseling & Development, 79, 387–397. doi: 10.1002/j.1556-6676.2001.tb01985.x

Lating, J. M., Barnett, J. E., & Horowitz, M. (2009). Increasing advocacy awareness within professional psychology training programs: The 2005 National Council of Schools and Programs of Professional Psychology Self-Study. Training and Education in Professional Psychology, 3, 106 –110. doi:10.1037/a0013662

London, M. (2010). Understanding social advocacy: An integrative model of motivation, strategy, and persistence in support of corporate social responsibility and social entrepreneurship. Journal of Management De- velopment, 29, 224 –245.

Myers, J. E., & Sweeney, T. J. (2004). Advocacy for the counseling profession: Results of a national survey. Journal of Counseling & Development, 82, 466 – 471. doi:10.1002/j.1556-6678.2004.tb00335.x

377PERCEIVED BARRIERS TO PUBLIC POLICY ADVOCACY

T hi

s do

cu m

en t i

s co

py ri

gh te

d by

th e

A m

er ic

an P

sy ch

ol og

ic al

A ss

oc ia

tio n

or o

ne o

f i ts

a lli

ed p

ub lis

he rs

. T

hi s

ar tic

le is

in te

nd ed

s ol

el y

fo r t

he p

er so

na l u

se o

f t he

in di

vi du

al u

se r a

nd is

n ot

to b

e di

ss em

in at

ed b

ro ad

ly .

 

 

Pfeiffer, S. M. (2007). Political giving by health professions. Advance: Newsletter of the Association for the Advancement of Psychology, 12.

Ratts, M., D’Andrea, M., & Arredondo, P. (2004). Social justice counsel- ing: “Fifth force” in field. Counseling Today, 28 –30.

Ratts, M., & Hutchins, A. M. (2009). ACA advocacy competencies: A social justice advocacy at the client/student level. Journal of Counseling & Development, 87, 269 –275. doi:10.1002/j.1556-6678.2009.tb00106.x

Toporek, R. L., Gerstein, L., Fouad, N., Roysircar, G., & Israel, T. (2006). Handbook for social justice in counseling psychology: Leadership, vi- sion, and action. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

Trusty, J., & Brown, D. (2005). Advocacy competencies for professional school counselors. Professional School Counseling, 8, 259 –265.

Waldmann, A. K., & Blackwell, T. L. (2010). Advocacy and accessibility standards in the new Code of Professional Ethics for rehabilitation counselors. Rehabilitation Counseling Bulletin, 53, 243–248. doi: 10.1177/0034355210368866

Received December 20, 2011 Revision received May 7, 2012

Accepted May 9, 2012 �

378 HEINOWITZ ET AL.

T hi

s do

cu m

en t i

s co

py ri

gh te

d by

th e

A m

er ic

an P

sy ch

ol og

ic al

A ss

oc ia

tio n

or o

ne o

f i ts

a lli

ed p

ub lis

he rs

. T

hi s

ar tic

le is

in te

nd ed

s ol

el y

fo r t

he p

er so

na l u

se o

f t he

in di

vi du

al u

se r a

nd is

n ot

to b

e di

ss em

in at

ed b

ro ad

ly .

Behavior Modification Slideshow

For this assignment, the student will take on the task of using behaviorism to assess and change a personal behavior utilizing the  tenants of either classical conditioning, operant conditioning or both.

Create a 12  slide PowerPoint presentation that provides an overview of the behavioral change.

Use the following guidelines to complete your presentation:

  1. Include a title, introduction, discussion, and reference slide (four slides minimum).
  2. Provide an overview of the definition of behavior and create an operational definition for the behavior the student wishes to change (consider using  the “Dead Man Test” to help define the behavior).
  3. Explain the desire for change, why is it important to incorporate this specific behavioral change.
  4. Track the behavior for three or more days documenting; how often the behavior occurs as well as the antecedents for the behavior and the  consequences.
  5. Explain the behavioral intervention, how will the student increase a desired behavior or decrease a maladaptive one?
  6. Track  the results of the behavioral change with the intervention in place,  how often does the behavior occur each day, how often did the reward or  punishment occur?
  7. Use a graph to compare the observation period from the experimental period.
  8. Document the results of the experiment; did change occur, what was the cause?

Behaviors  may include, but are not limited to: tardiness, smoking, drinking,  gambling, compulsive eating, social media addiction, interrupting, nail-biting, swearing, eating fast food, compulsive shopping, speaking with your mouth full, biting pens, or biting nails. You could also choose to  start a new habit: taking vitamins, applying sunscreen before going  outside, putting money in savings, exercise, etc.

Procrastination is NOT an appropriate behavior to choose for this assignment! You must choose something measurable!

Utilize  information from the textbook and the GCU library. Information from  non-peer-reviewed resources (e.g., simplypsychology.com, about.com, and  W  ikipedia) will not be accepted.

Include speaker notes below each content-related slide that represent what would be said if giving  the presentation in person. Expand upon the information included in the  slide and do not simply restate it (50 words per slide).

APA style is not required, but solid academic writing is expected.

This assignment uses a rubric. Please review the rubric prior to beginning the assignment to become familiar with the expectations for successful completion.

Benchmark Information
This benchmark assignment assesses the following programmatic competency.
BS Psychology, No Emphasis
BS Psychology, Emphasis in Performance and Sport Psychology
4.1 Implement strategies for self-management and self-improvement

Rubic_Print_Format

Course Code Class Code Assignment Title Total Points
Benchmark – Personal Behavior Modification
Criteria Percentage Unsatisfactory (0.00%) Less than Satisfactory (65.00%) Satisfactory (75.00%) Good (85.00%) Excellent (100.00%) Comments Points Earned
Content 70.0%
Definition of behavior and desire for change 15.0% Definition of behavior and desire for change is not present or not discernible to the reader. Essay does not demonstrate understanding of the topic. Definition of behavior and desire for change is incomplete or flawed. Essay demonstrates a poor understanding of the topic. Definition of behavior and desire for change is accurate and complete. Essay demonstrates a basic understanding of the topic. Definition of behavior and desire for change is thorough and well-reasoned. Essay demonstrates an understanding that extends beyond the surface of the topic. Definition of behavior and desire for change is thorough, well-reasoned, fully supported, and rich in detail. Essay demonstrates an exceptional understanding of the topic.
Behavior tracking including antecedents and consequences 20.0% Behavior tracking including antecedents and consequences is not present or not discernible to the reader. Essay does not demonstrate understanding of the topic. Behavior tracking including antecedents and consequences is incomplete or flawed. Essay demonstrates a poor understanding of the topic. Behavior tracking including antecedents and consequences is accurate and complete. Essay demonstrates a basic understanding of the topic. Behavior tracking including antecedents and consequences is thorough and well-reasoned. Essay demonstrates an understanding that extends beyond the surface of the topic. Behavior tracking including antecedents and consequences is thorough, well-reasoned, fully supported, and rich in detail. Essay demonstrates an exceptional understanding of the topic.
Intervention using behavioral techniques 15.0% Intervention using behavioral techniques is not present or not discernible to the reader. Essay does not demonstrate understanding of the topic. Intervention using behavioral techniques is incomplete or flawed. Essay demonstrates a poor understanding of the topic. Intervention using behavioral techniques is accurate and complete. Essay demonstrates a basic understanding of the topic. Intervention using behavioral techniques is thorough and well-reasoned. Essay demonstrates an understanding that extends beyond the surface of the topic. Intervention using behavioral techniques is thorough, well-reasoned, fully supported, and rich in detail. Essay demonstrates an exceptional understanding of the topic.
Results of behavioral intervention (C4.1) 20.0% Intervention using behavioral techniques is not present or not discernible to the reader. Essay does not demonstrate understanding of the topic. Intervention using behavioral techniques is incomplete or flawed. Essay demonstrates a poor understanding of the topic. Intervention using behavioral techniques is accurate and complete. Essay demonstrates a basic understanding of the topic. Intervention using behavioral techniques is thorough and well-reasoned. Essay demonstrates an understanding that extends beyond the surface of the topic. Intervention using behavioral techniques is thorough, well-reasoned, fully supported, and rich in detail. Essay demonstrates an exceptional understanding of the topic.
Organization, Effectiveness, and Format 30.0%
Layout 10.0% The layout is cluttered, confusing, and does not use spacing, headings, and subheadings to enhance the readability. The text is extremely difficult to read with long blocks of text, small point size for fonts, and inappropriate contrasting colors. Poor use of headings, subheadings, indentations, or bold formatting is evident. The layout shows some structure, but appears cluttered and busy or distracting with large gaps of white space or a distracting background. Overall readability is difficult due to lengthy paragraphs, too many different fonts, dark or busy background, overuse of bold, or lack of appropriate indentations of text. The layout uses horizontal and vertical white space appropriately. Sometimes the fonts are easy to read, but in a few places the use of fonts, italics, bold, long paragraphs, color, or busy background detracts and does not enhance readability. The layout background and text complement each other and enable the content to be easily read. The fonts are easy to read and point size varies appropriately for headings and text. The layout is visually pleasing and contributes to the overall message with appropriate use of headings, subheadings, and white space. Text is appropriate in length for the target audience and to the point. The background and colors enhance the readability of the text.
Language Use and Audience Awareness (includes sentence construction, word choice, etc.) 10.0% Inappropriate word choice and lack of variety in language use are evident. Writer appears to be unaware of audience. Use of primer prose indicates writer either does not apply figures of speech or uses them inappropriately. Some distracting inconsistencies in language choice (register) or word choice are present. The writer exhibits some lack of control in using figures of speech appropriately. Language is appropriate to the targeted audience for the most part. The writer is clearly aware of audience, uses a variety of appropriate vocabulary for the targeted audience, and uses figures of speech to communicate clearly. The writer uses a variety of sentence constructions, figures of speech, and word choice in distinctive and creative ways that are appropriate to purpose, discipline, and scope.
Mechanics of Writing (includes spelling, punctuation, grammar, language use) 5.0% Slide errors are pervasive enough that they impede communication of meaning. Frequent and repetitive mechanical errors distract the reader. Some mechanical errors or typos are present, but they are not overly distracting to the reader. Slides are largely free of mechanical errors, although a few may be present. Writer is clearly in control of standard, written, academic English.
Documentation of Sources (citations, footnotes, references, bibliography, etc., as appropriate to assignment and style) 5.0% Sources are not documented. Documentation of sources is inconsistent or incorrect, as appropriate to assignment and style, with numerous formatting errors. Sources are documented, as appropriate to assignment and style, although some formatting errors may be present. Sources are documented, as appropriate to assignment and style, and format is mostly correct. Sources are completely and correctly documented, as appropriate to assignment and style, and format is free of error.
Total Weightage 100%

Conceptual Skills In Earlychildhood Math

Teaching New Conceptual Knowledge or Skill
1. Write about a particular academic concept (e.g., math/sciences) or skill (e.g., social skill/behavior) which you would like the classroom to learn. Discuss how you would introduce teaching this concept and the barriers that you would likely encounter. Consider prior knowledge, learning processes/outcomes, behavioral/cognitive perspectives of learning, and/or working memory.
2. Discuss how you would have the child/classroom transfer this knowledge to real-life scenarios, particularly once they leave the classroom or graduate from school. Consider aspects of motivation and transfer.
3. How would you assess whether the concept was learned? Also, discuss any adaptations to the lesson plan if teaching the concept/skill in a culturally and linguistically diverse classroom.

Scoring rubric for Summary/References assignment (Worth 5 points total)
Abstract/summary/references written in MOST RECENT version of APA 2
Abstract/summary informs how you want to develop your topic 2
Minimum 4 total References: (at least 3 from scientific journals and class text book should be include) 1

It is double space and 6-7 pages ( not including title and reference page)

Writing clarity organization, and successful integration of the literature are considered when determining full credit for the above stated criteria.

Please No plagiarism.
As stated the book should be use as a reference if it makes you feel better you can start the paper over, but please make sure you answer the above questions. Since i already submitted a rough draft to the professor in regards of doing math as my topic I would like to stick in the area– If you want to do elementary math instead of kindergarten that is fine.

Here are some topic to discuss in this papaer:

Introducing teaching math concepts

Barries students run into

how to transfer knowledge into real life scenarios

how do you know the concept was learn… —> For better understanding just refer back to the question.

Instead of monday if you could please submit the paper to me by saturday morning est time.

Please lmk if you have any questions

Class is Educational Psychology

2

 

 

 

 

 

Teaching New Conceptual Knowledge or Skill: Kindergarten Math

 

 

Danielle D. Mills

Florida International University

EDP 3004:

Naylet LaRochelle, Ed.S.

June 1, 2020

 

Abstract

 

Math in kindergarten is all about the essentials. Students will be able to learn how to count, tally, and recognize numbers up to ten. Students will also be able to sort objects utilizing solid props. They will also be able to get familiar with the ideal and concepts of more and less ordinal numbers, basic addition and subtraction and creating patterns. The best approach to introduce this ideal is the top- down concept. Using the top down concept instructors will be able to select certain materials that is align with the subject for students to be able to engage. According to research studies instructive materials are valuable to the degree because they encourage students to think in critical thinking ways.

As a kindergarten math instructor your everyday lesson plan entails mostly informal components, some artistic patterns and music, some motivated by greed and or rivalry’s, some playful and completive and some originating and intellectual curiosity. We live in a world that revolves around technology therefore teaching this subject using technology would be great approach which could offer video content, and examples for practice. The long-term goal is to start preparing students to learn material that could be applied in real – life scenarios helping them reach the next level in their education.

When Introducing a topic, the teacher needs to look at multiple or different approaches which motivate learners and encourage them to learn it. In teaching mathematics, teachers need to have fluency with examples and terms as well as an understanding about the nature of mathematical proficiency and knowledge of mathematical reasoning. This knowledge of mathematical skills or mathematical knowledge for teaching (MKT), which is unique for teaching mathematical subjects, is defined as the mathematical knowledge required to assess the continuing tasks of teaching mathematics to learners [12]. When teaching mathematics using innovative approaches can motivate learners to pay attention in class. Researchers suggest that not only should teachers understand mathematics but also have knowledge of their learners and pedagogical skills.

Gardella [3] advises that learning mathematics begins with making links to previous concepts and use of language that is familiar to learners to allow them to internalize the concepts. Mosvold [24] also suggests that teaching should be linked to real life. Teachers should not focus on explaining the rules and definitions when introducing a new topic in the classroom but should try to consider more interesting alternatives. Ma and Papanastasiou [2] assert that involving different instructional methods to begin a new topic in mathematics can have a positive influence on learners’ mathematics performance. Ma’s [25] findings revealed that instructional methods which involve practical examples or story problems related to everyday life and learning by pairs or small groups on a project also had statistically significant positive effects on learner mathematics performance in various mathematical areas. Ma adds that learner-centred cooperative learning is also more appropriate than teacher-centred lecture instruction to set the stage for learning a new topic in mathematics. (Author) Cockett and Kilgour [26] assert that the use of techniques which enable children to break away from the traditional classroom setting and instructional style can increase the learners’ confidence in solving difficult mathematics tasks. Posing questions can be an effective tool to stimulate learners’ thinking when introducing a lesson or a concept. Questioning approaches play a crucial role in the quality of learning, given that, when asking questions, teachers enable their learners to reason and develop their level of thinking

 

 

 

References

Durwin, C. &. (2017). Ed Psych Modules. Illinois: Sage Publications Ltd. Ginsburg, H., Woods, T. A., & Hyson, M. (2014). Preparing Early Childhood Educators to Teach Math: Professional Development That Works. Baltimore: Brookes Publishing. Riveria, F. (2014). Teaching to the Math Common Core State Standards: Focus on Kinfergarten to grade 5. Rotterdam: SensePublishers. Rosales, A. (2015). Mathematizing: An emergent math curriculum approach for young children . St. Paul, MN: RedLeaf Press.

Observation Journal

SCS 100 Project 2: Observation Journal Guidelines and Rubric

Overview Your second longer-term assignment in this course is to complete an observation journal, where you will use the previous information collected in your comparison template to apply social scientific observations to help you to develop a question about the ads that a social scientist might ask. The work you do on this observation journal will directly support your work on your third course project, the final reflection, which is due later in the course.

This observation journal assignment will assess the following course outcome, which you focused on throughout Theme 2:

 

 Develop questions about fundamental aspects of human behavior that inform personal assumptions, beliefs and values using evidence from the social sciences

 

Prompt Your second course project is an observation journal. You have already gathered your advertisements and completed your comparison template to determine the social science approaches that are relevant to your ads. For this assignment you will use that information to write an observation journal that will ask you to draw conclusions from the ads and, eventually, devise a social science question that you might like to investigate. The critical elements of this assessment will be evaluated in your observation journal.

 

Specifically, the following critical elements must be addressed and will be graded using the rubric at the end of this document:

I. Explain why you chose these advertisements for social scientific and personal study. For instance, what aspects of them intrigued you and made you curious?

II. Explain the assumptions and observations about human interactions and behaviors you made about the advertisements. These are some questions you might want to consider in your explanation:

a. Who do you believe the audiences for the ads might be?

b. What messages do you think the ads are sending? c. What do you think the nature of the relationship is between or among the people in the ads? d. What relationship(s) do you see between or among the people and the product or service being advertised? e. How effective are the ads in influencing your own consumer decisions?

III. Identify topics in this course that are relevant to the human behaviors in your advertisements and explain how they are relevant. This is your social science evidence for your observations. For instance, what ideas and people have you studied so far that apply to your observations?

IV. Taking all of your observations and objective conclusions about human behavior in your advertisements into account, assume the role of a social scientist. What question would you ask about the advertisements that you, as a social scientist, could seek to answer? What observations and objective conclusions lead you to this question?

 

 

 

Supporting Work and Resources The observation journal is due in Learning Block 2-7. Throughout Theme 2, there are three opportunities to work directly on different elements of the observation journal.

 

1. In learning block 1-8, you submitted your comparison template. You will use this to create your observation journal. 2. In learning block 2-6, you participated in a discussion using the ads you chose in support of your summative work. You can use your answers from this

discussion when completing your observation journal. 3. In learning block 2-7, you will work to finalize your observation journal and submit it to your instructor for grading. This learning block also includes an

Observation Journal Checklist that you can use to ensure you have met all the requirements of this project. You can also review the Observation Journal Exemplar for guidance in how to complete this assignment. Your instructor is available to provide guidance and answer any questions you may have as you work to finalize your observation journal.

 

Rubric Guidelines for Submission: This submission will contain your completed observation journal. Submit your observation journal as a Microsoft Word document.

Critical Elements Exemplary (100%) Proficient (85%) Needs Improvement (55%) Not Evident (0%) Value

Chose Meets “Proficient” criteria and details demonstrate insight into the connection between the social scientific and the personal

Explains why advertisements were chosen for social scientific and personal study

Explains why advertisements were chosen, but social scientific and personal connections are overly generalized

Does not explain why advertisements were chosen

23.75

Assumptions and Observations

Meets “Proficient” criteria and details demonstrate a mature awareness of human interactions and behaviors

Explains the assumptions and observations about human interactions and behaviors made about the ads

Explains the assumptions and observations about human interactions and behaviors made about the ads, but is overly generalized

Does not explain the assumptions and observations about human interactions and behaviors made about the ads

23.75

Topics Meets “Proficient” criteria and connection between topics and ads shows a strong grasp of the social science evidence

Explains how topics from the course are relevant to the human behaviors in the ads

Explains how topics from the course are relevant to the human behaviors in the ads, but is overly generalized or has inaccuracies

Does not explain how topics from the course are relevant to the human behaviors in the ads

23.75

 

http://snhu-media.snhu.edu/files/course_repository/undergraduate/scs/scs100/Theme2/scs100_theme2_observation_journal_checklist.pdf
http://snhu-media.snhu.edu/files/course_repository/undergraduate/scs/scs100/Theme2/scs100_theme2_observation_journal_exemplar.pdf
http://snhu-media.snhu.edu/files/course_repository/undergraduate/scs/scs100/Theme2/scs100_theme2_observation_journal_exemplar.pdf

 

 

Question Meets “Proficient” criteria and connections between question, observations, and conclusions demonstrate insight into social scientific study

Poses a question a social scientist could seek to answer, supported by observations and conclusions about human behavior in the ads

Poses a question a social scientist could seek to answer, but lacks support from observations and conclusion about human behavior in the ads or is overly generalized

Does not pose a question a social scientist could seek to answer

23.75

Articulation of Response

Submission is free of errors related to citations, grammar, spelling, syntax, and organization and is presented in a professional and easy-to- read format

Submission has no major errors related to citations, grammar, spelling, syntax, or organization

Submission has major errors related to citations, grammar, spelling, syntax, or organization that negatively impact readability and articulation of main ideas

Submission has critical errors related to citations, grammar, spelling, syntax, or organization that prevent understanding of ideas

5

Total 100%