Knowledge of applied psychology: Assignments, papers, or projects from courses and/or an internship that demonstrates your ability to apply psychology to personal, social, and/or organizational problems.

 

This assignment requires you to gather a compilation of papers and projects from this course and previous courses and add them to your Professional Portfolio. The portfolio is designed to help you compile all you have learned throughout your undergraduate studies with the best examples of your work to represent what you are capable of doing when you graduate the program. This assignment also gives you a opportunity to reflect on what you have learned as a student of psychology, and to prepare to actualize your post-graduation goals. It will serve as a resource as you navigate the world of prospective employers and graduate admissions committees. The student portfolio is also a measure of effectiveness for the Argosy University baccalaureate program in Psychology.  As you develop professionally following graduation, you can add work samples and other pertinent materials, as well as modify existing documents, to reflect that development.

The Professional Portfolio includes two main sections.  A Professional Profile section that is completed by all students with either an Employment focus or a Graduate School focus, depending on your post-baccalaureate plans. The second section holds your Professional Work Samples demonstrating your abilities, skills, and competencies across a range of tasks and objectives.  This section also includes a Final Evaluation of your work samples, yourself, and the psychology program.

The Professional Profile section summarizes your career planning and professional goals. You have already compiled the documents for this section in Modules 1 and 2 of this course. It contains the following documents:

  1. Cover Letter as a way to introduce yourself to a potential employer This includes details about yourself and why you feel you are qualified for the position. (Employment).

OR

Personal Statement describing your immediate and long-term career goals, including an explanation of why these goals are appropriate in terms of your knowledge, skills, personal characteristics, values, and experiences (Graduate School).

Note: The personal statement is attached to the Curriculum Vitae (Graduate School), and the Cover Letter is attached to the Resume (Employment), so take the time to be creative and not repeat yourself.

  1. Resume with list of References that is current, accurate, and professional in appearance. (Employment).

OR

Curriculum Vitae with list of References that is current, accurate and professional in appearance. (Graduate School)

  1. OPTIONAL: A copy of your Transcript(s)

The Professional Work Samples section demonstrates your performance and mastery of Argosy University’s program outcomes/competencies for the Psychology major. Select 3-5 documents to serve as samples of your best work in the program. Together, they should illustrate your mastery of all the 7 program outcomes listed below. Examples of the type of work that would illustrate each outcome is provided below, as well. The 3-5 documents should be samples of your work that you completed in previous courses as an undergraduate student.

These outcomes are listed below, along with the assignments that could be included to demonstrate mastery of the outcome:

  • Cognitive abilities (critical thinking and information literacy): Assignments or projects that demonstrate problem solving, analysis, synthesis, appropriate use of information resources, etc.
  • Research skills: A copy of a research proposal, a survey developed, etc.
  • Communication skills (written and oral): A topic paper, presentation outline, or PowerPoint that demonstrates your written and oral communication skills.
  • Ethics & Diversity awareness: Papers that demonstrate your understanding and/or analysis of ethical and diversity issues in psychology.
  • Knowledge of foundations of the field: Assignments, papers, or projects that demonstrate your understanding of basic concepts, theories, and empirical findings in one or more of the domains of psychology, including biological, cognitive, developmental, personality, and social.
  • Knowledge of applied psychology: Assignments, papers, or projects from courses and/or an internship that demonstrates your ability to apply psychology to personal, social, and/or organizational problems.
  • Interpersonal Effectiveness: PowerPoint presentations, videos of your presentations that demonstrate your ability to communicate effectively, appreciate diversity and cultural sensitivity and awareness of your impact on others.

Once you have assembled your work samples, you will write a 700-1000 word narrative to serve as a preface to the Professional Work Samples section. This narrative should consist of a clear and thoughtful analysis of how each work sample reveals your strengths and areas for improvement, according to each of the seven areas addressed in the program outcomes/competencies (listed above). Towards this purpose, you will complete the Student Self Appraisal of Learningfirst, and then use that to generate the preface . Make sure you also address how you intend to strengthen your competencies in all areas as you graduate onto the next step of your professional career. On which areas will you focus most and why?

This preface should also include an evaluation of the Psychology program as well as suggestions for the program that will enable it to help future Psychology majors develop these strengths and overcome these weaknesses in the future.

Enter each of these components (Professional Profile, Professional Work Samples, with self- evaluation) into the Professional Profile Template.

Assignment 2 Grading Criteria
Maximum Points
Compiled all the elements of the Portfolio using the template, in a neat and professional manner. Included at least 3 work samples.
10
Completed the SSAL and included it in the Portfolio.
10
Submitted a 700-1000 word, clear and thoughtful analysis of how each of the 3-5 work samples reveal your strengths and areas for improvement, according to each of the seven areas addressed in the program outcomes/competencies.
30
Addressed how you intend to strengthen your competencies in all areas as you graduate and which areas you would focus most on, and why.
15
Included an evaluation of the Psychology program along with suggestions to enable it to help future Psychology majors develop these strengths and overcome these weaknesses in the future.
15
Wrote in a clear, concise, and organized manner; demonstrated ethical scholarship in accurate representation and attribution of sources, displayed accurate spelling, grammar, and punctuation.
20
Total:

Which of the following statements regarding abuse is correct?

Question

Top of Form

Quiz 1 Wicks

Return to Assessment List

Part 1 of 1 – 100.0 Points

Question 1 of 25 4.0 Points

Which is least likely to be considered an indication of problem behavior in youth?

A.A behavior is excessively intense.

B.A behavior is qualitatively atypical.

C.A behavior is unusual but of no harm to anyone.

D.A behavior is exhibited in inappropriate settings.

Question 2 of 25 4.0 Points

Which of the following is true regarding preschooler mental health?

A. Preschoolers have fewer problems than older children.

B. Preschoolers have more problems, but tend to outgrow them.

C. The problems of preschoolers occur at the same rate as older children.

D. Secular trends indicate that the risks for young children and mental illness are decreasing.

Question 3 of 25 4.0 Points

When the term syndrome is used in reference to a behavioral disturbance, it suggests that the disorder

A. has a biological cause.

B. has a psychological cause.

C. occurs mostly in early childhood.

D. is characterized by a group of symptoms.

Question 4 of 25 4.0 Points

With regard to childhood behavioral problems and age of youth,

A. little relationship exists between age and the occurrence of problems.

B. sometimes what seems to be a relationship between age and the occurrence of a specific disorder may actually reflect the time at which the disorder is first identified.

C. almost all behavior problems of young people appear in some form by age 6.

D. children cannot be diagnosed with a specific disorder until they are at least 8 years old.

Question 5 of 25 4.0 Points

Human development is best viewed as

A. always occurring in stages.

B. changes in individuals due to environmental influences.

C. change in persons over time due to the interactions of many variables.

D. quantitative rather than qualitative growth.

Question 6 of 25 4.0 Points

If a disorder occurs only in the presence of Factor X but only when Factor Y or Factor Z are present, we might suspect that Factor X is a _____ cause of the disorder.

A. necessary but not sufficient

B. sufficient but not necessary

C. necessary and sufficient

D. neither necessary nor sufficient

Question 7 of 25 4.0 Points

Peter got along quite well as a child, but in adolescence he associated with a “bad” crowd, took drugs, and suffered academically. At 21 years of age, he appears to have put aside these behaviors and has successfully returned to college. Peter’s development is following which of the five adolescent developmental pathways described in the text?

A. Unstable adaptation

B. Unstable maladaptation

C. Decline of adaptation

D. Temporal maladaptation

Question 8 of 25 4.0 Points

Resilience is best defined as

A. a person’s characteristics that protect him or her from negative outcomes.

B. characteristics of the environment that protect a person from negative outcomes.

C. one or more factors that work with risk factors to produce a disorder.

D. one or more factors that protect a person in the presence of risk factors for a disorder.

Question 9 of 25 4.0 Points

Billy and Kyle are both evaluated at a child assessment clinic at age 6. Billy comes from an affluent, loving and intact family. He is meeting all developmental tasks and is well adjusted. Kyle is living in poverty with his maternal grandmother after being abandoned by his parents. He is scoring in the clinically significant range on measures that test for behavioral problems. Years later, both boys are re-evaluated and both are found to be well adjusted and developing normally. Kyle reports that he became active in sports and was mentored by caring adults over the years. This is an example of

A. ordinary magic of resilience.

B. multifinality.

C. unstable, maladaptive development.

D. nonnormative development.

Question 10 of 25 4.0 Points

Heterotypic continuity refers to the continuance

A. of risk factors across the lifespan.

B. of resilience factors across the lifespan.

C. of a problem in the same form over time.

D. of a problem, with the problem changing in form over time.

Question 11 of 25 4.0 Points

In the Strange Situation Kevin does not use his mother as a resource. He does not seem distressed when she leaves and ignores her when she returns. Which attachment style is Kevin exhibiting?

A. Secure

B. Insecure resistant

C. Insecure avoidant

D. Disorganized

Question 12 of 25 4.0 Points

Sandy cries easily, has tantrums when facing novel situations, and is highly reactive to stress. Which temperament type best describes Sandy?

A. Easy

B. Slow–to-warm-up

C. Difficult

D. Disorganized

Question 13 of 25 4.0 Points

According to models of social cognitive processing, which is central in mediating children’s experiences and their behavior?

A. Their interaction with their parents

B. Their interpretation of their experiences

C. Their earlier attachment to their parents

D. Their temperamental tendencies

Question 14 of 25 4.0 Points

Mr. and Mrs. Smith have recently adopted Jimmy, age 9. They do not know much about his birth history. They are concerned because Jimmy exhibits slow growth and poor motor skills. He also has small eyes and a thin upper lip. Finally, Jimmy seems to have difficulty learning and is quite hyperactive. His pediatrician suspects that

A. Jimmy was exposed to lead early in life.

B. Jimmy’s mother had Rubella while pregnant with Jimmy.*

C. Jimmy’s mother drank while pregnant with Jimmy.

D. Jimmy was born breech.

Question 15 of 25 4.0 Points

Many complex human characteristics, such as intelligence and behaviors implicated in behavior disorders, are believed to be associated with

A. a single gene.

B. a single gene in combination with environmental influences.

C. multiple genes.

D. multiple genes in combination with environmental influences.

Question 16 of 25 4.0 Points

Michael inherits a genetic propensity for high-activity level from his parents and the parents have created a family environment that is highly active. This is an example of a _______ gene-environment correlation.

A. active

B. passive

C. reactive

D. redundant

Question 17 of 25 4.0 Points

Attention, memory, concept formation, strategies to mentally manipulate information, and problem solving are examples of

A. instrumental processes.

B. observational processes.

C. cognitive processes

D. unconscious processes

Question 18 of 25 4.0 Points

In an ecological model of development, _______ contexts would be expected to have relatively more direct impact.

A. cultural

B. distal

C. proximal

D. pathologica.

Question 19 of 25 4.0 Points

According to the book, the definition of _________ is probably the most difficult and controversial.

A. emotional maltreatment

B. neglect

C. physical abuse

D. sexual abuse

Question 20 of 25 4.0 Points

Which of the following children is at the highest risk for abuse?

A.A one year-old with physical disabilities

B.A 12 year-old from a large family

C.A 17 year-old with failing grades

D.A 13 year-old Black male living in an impoverished neighborhood

Question 21 of 25 4.0 Points

Which of the following variables is thought to contribute to a youngster’s adjustment to divorce?

A. The youngster’s prior level of adjustment

B. Individual characteristics of parents

C. The youngster’s prior level of adjustment and individual characteristics of parents

D. The youngster’s prior level of adjustment, individual characteristics of parents, and ethnic differences

Question 22 of 25 4.0 Points

Which of the following statements regarding the impact of poverty on youth is accurate?

A. Rates of poverty are equal across racial groups.

B. Single female-headed households are at higher risk for poverty than married households.

C. Childrearing practices do not vary by social class.

D. Poverty is most damaging to middleschoolers.

Question 23 of 25 4.0 Points

The term acculturation refers to

A. the elimination of cultural influences so as to understand the effect of individual variables.

B. changes in culture resulting from different cultures coming in contact with each other.

C. the attempt to provide cultural experiences, such as music and art to at-risk children.

D. interventions to overcome the negative impact of a youngster’s cultural background.

Question 24 of 25 4.0 Points

Which of the following is an example of molecular family stability?

A. Living in a family with no divorce history

B. Living in the same home and never moving

C. Consistency in who lives in the home

D. Having a regular bed time

Question 25 of 25 4.0 Points

Which of the following statements regarding abuse is correct?

A. Physical abuse is harder to detect than other forms of maltreatment.

B. Sexual abuse of boys is more common than that of girls.

C. Refusing special education when needed can be considered neglect.

D. According to the book, in 2009 there were less than 200,000 cases of maltreatment reported in the U.S.

Distinguish among age-graded, history-graded, and non-normative influences on lifespan development. Cite an example of each in Sofie’s story at the beginning of this chapter.

 

Assignment:

Essay: Write a 1000-1800 word essay addressing each of the following points/questions. Be sure to completely answer all the questions for each questions. Separate each section in your paper with a clear heading that allows your professor to know which question you are addressing in that section of your paper. Support your ideas with at least three (3) citations in your essay. Make sure to reference the citations using the APA writing style for the essay. The cover page and reference page do not count towards the minimum word amount.

1. What strengths and limitations do the clinical (or case study) method and ethnography have in common?

2. Distinguish among age-graded, history-graded, and non-normative influences on lifespan development. Cite an example of each in Sofie’s story at the beginning of this chapter.

3. What is epigenesis, and how does it differ from range of reaction and genetic–environmental correlation? Provide an example of epigenesis.

4. Links between family and community foster development throughout the lifespan. Cite several examples from our discussion that support this idea.

5. Why is genetic counseling called a communication process? Who should seek it?

6. Using your knowledge of X-linked inheritance, explain why males are more vulnerable than females to miscarriage, infant death, genetic disorders, and other problems.

7. Why is the period of the embryo regarded as the most dramatic prenatal period? Why is the fetal period called the “growth and finishing” phase?

8. What functions does REM sleep serve in young infants? Can sleep tell us anything about the health of the newborn’s central nervous system? Explain.

Please select the two correct statements that refute the genetic-maturational perspective’s argument that biological factors determine how children react and regulate their emotions.

ASSIGNMENT

 

Chomsky’s Theory of Language Development discusses “critical periods” for learning language.  Following from this theory, disruptions during critical periods should negatively affect the development of language.

Unfortunately, there are some examples from real life to demonstrate this hypothesis.  Please link to and read the following regarding both a very recent and an historic case:

http://abcnews.go.com/Health/story?id=4758945&page=1

Here are some additional, optional resources on Genie:

http://documentarystorm.com/secret-of-the-wild-child/

http://www.pbs.org/wgbh/nova/transcripts/2112gchild.html

Obviously, these are both horrific cases of child abuse.

1.)  What does “Genie’s” final outcome tell us about language (and emotional) development?

2.)  What cues can educators take from these tragic cases?

Introduction

This lesson will explore the emotional and communication development of children. Firstly, we will discuss the theories of emotional development. We will then look at how emotion develops in two main stages: primary emotions which include joy, anger and fear, and secondary emotions which comprise the self-conscious emotions. We will then move onto attachment theory where we investigate how the different kinds of caregiver-infant relationships either create secure or insecure attachments, and the impact of these attachments on child development. In the second part of the lesson, we will explore language and communication development. We will cover how this development is socially facilitated, as well as the components of language and communication. Lastly, we will discuss the social use of language.

Emotional Development

‹›

· Emotions

Emotions have many important functions and have a significant impact on child development. Emotions are internal responses to the environment, that are accompanied by physiological and behavioral changes. For instance, sadness may be accompanied by a change in heart rate and release of cortisol, a stress hormone. Learning how to interpret other’s emotions is also a key aspect of development.

Development of Emotional Expression

While most mothers agree that they can detect emotions in the first month of their baby’s life (Johnson, Emde, Pannabecker, Stenberg, & Davis, 1982), the Maximally Discriminative Facial Movement (MAX) coding system developed by Izard, Fantauzzo, Castle, Haynes and Slomine (1995), determines infant emotions based on their body movements and facial expressions.

PRIMARY EMOTIONS

JOY

GENDER AND NATIONALITY DIFFERENCES

LAUGHING

Fear and Other Emotions

· FEAR

· STRANGER DISTRESS

· INTERACTIONS

· OTHER EMOTIONS

Fear is the second predominant primary emotion. Fear of strangers emerges around the same time that infants begin to show positive emotion to familiar people. According to Sroufe (1996), at around three months, infants begin to be wary when they are exposed to new situations because they have difficulty assimilating and comprehending the unfamiliar. From around seven months, this wariness turns into outright fear and distress.

Knowledge Check

1

Question 1

Please select the two correct statements that refute the genetic-maturational perspective’s argument that biological factors determine how children react and regulate their emotions.

 

The   rate of infant smiling is related to the rate of caregiver stimulation.

 

Babies   begin to smile at 46 weeks from conception, whether they were born premature   or full-term.

 

Stranger   distress is not universal since it does not occur in cultures in which   caregiving is shared among multiple relatives.

 

From   about eight weeks, babies begin to look a lot at the mouth and respond to   smiles.

I don’t know

One attempt

Submit answer

You answered 0 out of 0 correctly. Asking up to 1.

Secondary Emotions

Secondary emotions function to identify and coordinate the role of the individual’s responsibility in a situation that involves other factors and/or people. Secondary emotion are self-conscious emotions that describe the individual’s perception of their relatively superior or inferior position. Secondary emotions emerge from around the second year (Saarni et al., 2006).

Emotional reactions differ from child to child, and are a consequence of temperament and environmental factors – particularly parental modeling. High negative emotionality results in more adjustment difficulties, depression and behavioral problems, while children with positive emotionality have high self-esteem, social competence and less adjustment issues.

PRIDE AND SHAME

GUILT

JEALOUSY

JEALOUSY OF PARENTAL ATTENTION

Identifying Emotions in Others

Infants initially learn to interpret other’s emotions by observing caregiver facial expressions. Research suggests that babies recognize caregiver joy before they are able to recognize anger – similar to how babies first express joy and only later express anger (Izard et al., 1995). As children get older, they more accurately discern between genuine and inauthentic smiles (Del Giudice & Colle, 2007).

Experiences Impact Emotions

Children’s early experiences impact their ability to recognize emotions. For example, children who have experienced high levels of threat and hostility recognize anger more quickly and sadness more slowly than other children (Pollak & Sinha, 2002). Children from cultures that value group harmony and focus on other’s feelings, such as China and Mexico, are more capable than Australian and U.S. children at recognizing other’s emotions (Cole & Tan, 2007).

Emotional Regulation

· SELF-REGULATION

· EMOTIONAL RULES

· CONFLICTING EMOTIONS

· FAMILY ROLE

An important facet of development is learning to regulate and take control of one’s emotional expression. Infants learn that sucking their thumbs is soothing, while older children learn to avoid frightening situations and distract themselves. As children grow up, increased self-control modulates the intensity, frequency and variability of their emotions, and predicts later adjustment (Fox & Calkins, 2003; Saarni et al., 2006). For example, it is normal for two-year olds to have tantrums, but tantrums in older children and adults are not considered normal or healthy.

Watch this video on early childhood self-regulation.

Knowledge Check

1

Question 1

Caregivers have the following two responsibilities:

 

To   help children understand and speak about their own and other’s emotions.

 

To   make their babies smile as often as possible because this shows that the baby   is happy and healthy.

 

To   ensure children never experience negative emotions like jealousy, shame and   fear, because negative emotions impact development.

 

To   ensure that parent-child and parent-parent relationships in the home are   supportive and cooperative.

I don’t know

One attempt

Submit answer

You answered 0 out of 0 correctly. Asking up to 1.

Attachment

Attachment is the emotional bond between the infant and caregivers, and is foundational to the rest of the child’s development. Psychoanalytic and learning theory associate attachment with the satisfaction of the infant’s primary drive of hunger. The cognitive developmental view proposes that attachment teaches infants that others continue to exist even when they cannot be seen. The ethological perspective describes that children and caregivers are biologically programmed to respond to each other and develop a mutual attachment.

Attachment processes continue into adolescence, and determine how adolescents gain independence, form relationships with others and perceive the world. Furthermore, these patterns are generally repeated with our own children when we become parents. This is referred to as intergenerational continuity.

Some people are resilient enough to overcome dysfunctional attachments, and develop secure, satisfying relationships with their spouses and children. These people are referred to as earned secure individuals (Paley, Cox, Burchinal, & Payne, 1999). Professional help can improve parent-child relationships.

SECURE BASE

SECURE ATTACHMENT

INSECURE-AVOIDANT ATTACHMENT

INSECURE-RESISTANT ATTACHMENT

INSECURE-DISORGANIZED ATTACHMENT

Impact Of Attachment on Development

· ATTACHMENT

· ATTACHMENT AND SOCIAL BEHAVIOR

· SENSE OF SELF

· DAYCARE

How do you think neural plasticity and the quality of attachment relate?

Research shows that attachments to both mother and father are equally important (Parke & Buriel, 2006). Secure attachment, caregiver responsiveness and good parent-child relationships are related to more complex cognitive development and higher academic achievement and participation (Jacobsen & Hofmann 1997; Stams, Juffer, & van Ijzendoorn, 2002).

Knowledge Check

1

Question 1

Please select the correct statement.

 

Children   with insecure-disorganized attachment are probably angry with their   caregiver’s inconsistent availability.

 

Caregivers   who are unavailable, inconsistent, intrusive, or frightening tend to create   attachment dysfunction in children.

 

Children   who are sent to daycare are more prone to attachment disorders.

 

Children   who have attachment disorders will probably never be able to form healthy   relationships.

I don’t know

One attempt

Submit answer

You answered 0 out of 0 correctly. Asking up to 1.

Language and Communication

While humans are genetically predisposed to learning language, social support is crucial when children learn to speak and communicate. Language is a complex system of rules that allows us to send messages to one another through words, symbols and behaviors. We use it to relate, express ourselves, influence, inform others and achieve goals. Language is important to teach children how to regulate their emotions, control their actions and organize their thinking (Parke & Gauvain, 2009).

Communication competence allows children to express themselves in meaningful and culturally relevant ways. Communication is a two-way process, whereby productive language refers to the production of communication, while receptive language refers to understanding other’s communication.

PHONOLOGY

SEMANTICS

GRAMMAR

PRAGMATICS

Language Development Theories

We will explore each of these components in more detail, but first, we will discuss the theories of language development.

· LEARNING PERSPECTIVE

· NATIVIST PERSPECTIVE

· INTERACTIONIST PERSPECTIVE

The learning perspective proposes that children learn language through caregivers who positively reinforce infant babbling that most sounds like speech, and because children learn through imitating and generalizing what they observe and hear from others.

Facilitating Language Development

‹›

· Language Acquisition Support System (LASS)

The language acquisition support system (LASS) is the environment provided by caregivers and other people like siblings, in which children learn language (Bruner, 1983). Nonverbal games such as peekaboo have predictable patterns which may lay the foundation for language and communication rules – for example, turn-taking. Parents and siblings usually talk to the child throughout these games, commenting on the child’s actions and what is occurring, and anticipating the child’s needs, thus laying the foundation for language.

Antecedents of Language

· PRELINGUISTIC COMMUNICATION

· RHYTHMIC ORGANIZATION

· CRYING, BABBLING, AND COOING

· CULTURAL CONSISTENCIES

Since communication is more than just verbal language, prelinguistic communication such as facial expressions, gestures and movement are important precursors (Adamson, 1995). From three months, infants begin to respond to caregivers with smiles, movements, sounds and gestures, from six months they begin to make pointing gestures at objects and from one year they can follow someone else’s pointing gesture (Fogel, 1993).

Semantic Development

Children understand more than they are able to express. One-and-a-half-year olds typically understand between 50 to100 words and begin to say their first words. Two-year olds understand around 900 words, and six-year olds understand around 8000 words. This increase in vocabulary is referred to as the naming explosion (Bloom, Lifter & Broughton, 1985).

Children learn object words first, probably because it is easier to understand the relationship between the object, concept and word, than understanding actions and abstract concepts (Gentner, 1982). Action words are more easily learnt when it is an action the child can perform, such as running and jumping. Overextension of words occurs when one word is used for many objects. For instance, all animals may be referred to as cat. Underextension occurs when a word is used in a very limited way, for instance if the word cat is used to identify only black cats.

Grammar

The child’s leap from using single words to full sentences is rapid. Children begin to communicate by using single words that seem to communicate full ideas – for example, if a child says ‘me’ the parent may realize the child is really saying ‘I want to do this myself’. This is referred to as a holophrase.

· TWO-YEAR OLDS

· THREE-YEAR OLDS

Two-year olds begin to use telegraphic speech or two-word sentences that contain only the words needed to convey the intended meaning – usually nouns, adjectives and verbs. For example, ‘me play’. This also occurs in two-year olds who use sign language. At this stage, children learn about the correct plural forms, and may overgeneralize rules they have learned. For instance, the plural form -s may be applied to all words: ‘mouses’ instead of ‘mice.’

Learning the Social Use of Language

Since language functions to help us express ourselves, and influence and relate to others, it is a social phenomenon. Pragmatics are thus crucial rules about what kind of communication is appropriate in specific situations.

Children generally begin to learn the first, second and third rules by around the age of two (Dunn, 1988; Wellman & Lempers, 1977). Miller and Sperry (1987) add that children need to learn how, where, when and to whom to express negative feelings such as anger and sadness. As with other social skills, children learn pragmatics from observation, listening, imitation and instruction.

According to Glucksberg, Kraus & Higgins (1975), children must learn the following pragmatics respectively:

RULE 1

RULE 2

RULE 3

RULE 4

RULE 5

RULE 6

Bilingualism

· BILINGUAL HOMES

· NOT A DETRIMENT

· COMPETENCE IN BOTH

Many children grow up learning two languages. In some cases, children learn two languages simultaneously, where perhaps one parent speaks to the child in one language, while the other parent speaks to the child in another language. In other cases, languages may be learned sequentially, where the first language is learned at home and the second at school.

Knowledge Check

1

Question 1

Which of the following utterances is most typical of a two-year old?

 

Apple.

 

Mommy!   Sweetie me.

 

Mommy   doesn’t understand me.

I don’t know

One attempt

Submit answer

You answered 0 out of 0 correctly. Asking up to 1.

Conclusion

In this lesson, we covered the emotional development of children, attachment theory and the development of language and communication in children. We began the lesson by discussing the theoretical approaches to emotional development, and then moved on to exploring the development of emotional expression, where we discussed primary and secondary emotions, and emotional regulation. We then investigated how the caregiving style impacts the quality of attachment between children and caregivers, and the impact attachment has on child development. Thereafter, we looked at language and communication development. We briefly explored the main language development theories, and then looked at how language development can be facilitated. Lastly, we looked at the components of language and how they come together to be used socially.

KEY TERMS

References

Adamson, L. B. (1995). Communication development during infancy. Madison, WI: Brown & Benchmark.

Ainsworth, M. D. (1973). The development of infant-mother attachment. In B. Caldwell & H. Ricciuti (Eds.), Review of child development research (Vol. 3, pp. 1–94). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Bialystok, E. (1999). Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind. Child Development, 70, 636–644.

Bloom, L., Lifter, K., & Broughton, J. (1985). The convergence of early cognition and language in the second year of life: Problems in conceptualization and measurement. In M. Barrett (Ed.), Single word speech (pp. 149–181). London, UK: Wiley.

Bruner, J. (1983). Children’s talk. New York, NY: Norton.

Clarke-Stewart, K. A., & Allhusen, V. D. (2002). Nonparental caregiving. In M. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting (2nd ed., pp. 215–252). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Cole, P. M., & Tan, P. Z. (2007). Emotion socialization from a cultural perspective. In J. E. Grusec & P. Hastings (Eds.), Handbook of socialization (pp. 516–542). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Contreras, J. M., Kerns, K., Weimer, B. L., Gentzler, A. L., & Tomich, P. L. (2000). Emotional regulation as a mediator of association between mother-child attachment and peer relationships in middle childhood. Journal of Family Psychology, 14, 111–124.

Deacon, S. H., Wade-Woolley, L., & Kirby, J. (2007). Crossover: The role of morphological awareness in French immersion children’s reading. Developmental Psychology, 43, 732–746.

Del Giudice, M., & Colle, L. (2007). Differences between children and adults in the recognition of enjoyment smiles. Developmental Psychology, 43, 796–803.

Denham, S. A., Bassett, H. H., & Wyatt, T. (2007). The socialization of emotional competence. In J. E. Grusec, & P. Hastings (Eds.), Handbook of socialization (pp. 516–542). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

de Villiers, P. A., & de Villiers, J. G. (1992). Language development. In M. E. Lamb & M. H. Bornstein (Eds.), Developmental psychology: An advanced textbook (3rd ed., pp. 313–373). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Diaz, R. M. (1985). Bilingual cognitive development: Addressing three gaps in current research. Child Development, 56, 1376–1388.

Dittrichova, J. (1969). The development of premature infants. In R. J. Robinson (Ed.), Brain and early development. London, UK: Academic Press.

Dunn, J. (1988). The beginnings of social understanding. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Feinman, S., & Lewis, M. (1983). Social referencing at ten months: A second-order effect on infants’ responses to strangers. Child Development, 54, 878–887.

Fogel, A. (1993). Developing through relationships: Origins of communication, self, and culture. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Fox, N. A., & Calkins, S. (2003). The development of self-control of emotions: Intrinsic and external influences. Motivation and Emotion, 27, 7–26.

Gentner, D. (1982). Why nouns are learned before verbs: Linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning. In S. A. Kuczaj II (Ed.), Language development: Vol. 2. Language, thought, and culture (pp. 301–332). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Glucksberg, S., Krauss, R., & Higgins, E. T. (1975). The development of referential communication skills. In F. D. Horowitz (Ed.), Review of child development research (Vol. 4, pp. 305–345). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Goncz, L., & Kodzopeljic, J. (1991). Exposure to two languages in the preschool period: Metalinguistic development and the acquisition of reading. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 12, 137–142.

Harter, S., & Buddin, B. J. (1987). Children’s understanding of the simultaneity of two emotions: A five-stage developmental acquisition sequence. Developmental Psychology, 23, 388–399.

Harter, S. (2006). The self. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Series Eds.), & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (6th ed., Vol. 3, pp. 505–570). New York, NY: Wiley.

Izard, C. E., Fantauzzo, C. A., Castle, J. M., Haynes, O. M., & Slomine, B. S. (1995). The morphological stability and social validity of infants’ facial expressions. Unpublished manuscript, University of Delaware.

Jacobsen, T., & Hofmann, V. (1997). Children’s attachment representations: Longitudinal relations to school behavior and academic competency in middle childhood and adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 33, 703–710.

Johnson, W., Emde, R. N., Pannabecker, B., Stenberg, C., & Davis, M. (1982). Maternal perception of infant emotion from birth through 18 months. Infant Behaviorand Development, 5 , 313–322.

LaFrance, M., Hecht, M. A., & Levy Paluck, E. (2003). The contingent smile: A meta-analysis of sex differences in smiling. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 305–334.

Lewis, M. (2000). Self-conscious emotions: Embarrassment, pride, shame, and guilt. In M. Lewis & J. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 623–636). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Lewis, M., Alessandri, S., & Sullivan, M. W. (1992). Differences in shame and pride as a function of children’s gender and task difficulty. Child Development, 63, 630–638.

Lewis, M., & Michalson, L. (1985). Children’s emotions and moods. New York, NY: Plenum Press.

Mangelsdorf, S., Watkins, S., & Lehn, L. (1991, April). The role of control in the infant’s appraisal of strangers. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Seattle, Washington.

Miller, P., & Sperry, L. L. (1987). The socialization of anger and aggression. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 1–31.

Morgan, G. A., & Ricciuti, H. (1969). Infants’ responses to strangers during the first year. In B. M. Foss (Ed.), Determinants of infant behavior (Vol. 4, pp. 253–272). London, UK: Methuen.

Paley, B., Cox, M. J., Burchinal, M. R., & Payne, C. C. (1999). Attachment and family functioning: Comparison of spouses with continuous-secure, earned-secure, dismissing and preoccupied attachment stances. Journal of Family Psychology, 13, 580–597.

Parke, R., & Buriel, R. (2006). Socialization in the family: Ethnic and ecological perspectives. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Series Eds.), & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional and personality development (6th ed., pp. 429–504). New York, NY: Wiley.

Parke, R., & Gauvain, M. (2009).Child Psychology: A contemporary viewpoint  (7th ed.). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Pearson, B. Z., Fernandez, S. C., Lewedeg, V., & Oller, D. K. (1997). The relation of input factors of lexical learning by bilingual infants (ages 8 to 30 months). Applied Psycholinguistics, 18, 41–58.

Pearson, B. Z., Fernandez, S. C., & Oller, D. K. (1993). Lexical development in bilingual infants and toddlers: Comparison to monolingual norms. Language Learning, 43, 93–120.

Petitto, L. A., & Marentette, P. (1991). Babbling in the manual mode: Evidence for the ontogeny of language. Science, 251, 1493–1496.

Pollak, S. D., & Sinha, P. (2002). Effects of early experience on children’s recognition of facial displays of emotion. Developmental Psychology, 38, 784–791.

Saarni, C., Campos, J. J., & Camras, L. (2006). Emotional development. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Series Eds.), & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.). Handbook of child psychology: Vol 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 226–299). New York, NY: Wiley.

Schneider, B. H., Atkinson, L., & Tardif, C. (2001). Child-parent attachment and children’s peer relations: A quantitative review. Developmental Psychology, 37, 86–100.

Sokolov, J. L. (1993). A local contingency analysis of the fine-tuning hypothesis. Developmental Psychology, 29, 1008–1023.

Sroufe, L. A. (1996). Emotional development: The organization of emotional life in the early years. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Sroufe, L. A., Waters, E., & Matas, L. (1974). Contextual determinants of infant affectional response. In M. Lewis & L. Rosenblum (Eds.), Origins of fear (pp. 49–72). New York, NY: Wiley.

Stams, G. J. M., Juffer, F., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2002). Maternal sensitivity, infant attachment and temperament in early childhood predict adjustment in middle childhood: The case of adopted children and their biologically unrelated parents. Developmental Psychology, 38, 806–821.

Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H., & Baumwell, L. (2001). Maternal responsiveness and children’s achievement of language milestones. Child Development, 72, 748–767.

Thevenin, D. M., Eilers, R. E., Oller, D. K., & LaVoie, L. (1985). Where’s the drift in babbling drift? A cross-linguistic study. Applied Psycholinguistics, 6, 3–15.

Thompson, R. A. (2006). The development of the person: Social understanding, relationships, self, conscience. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Series Eds.), & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (6th ed, Vol 3, pp. 24–98). New York, NY: Wiley.

Tomasello, M. (2006). Acquiring metalinguistic constructions. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Series Eds.), & D. Kuhn & R. Siegler (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 2, 6th ed., pp. 255–298). New York, NY: Wiley.

Tronick, E. Z., Messinger, D. S., Weinberg, M. K., Lester, B. M., LaGasse, L., Seifer, R., et al. (2005). Cocaine exposure is associated with subtle compromises of infants’ and mothers’ social-emotional behavior and dyadic features of their interaction in the face still face paradigm. Developmental Psychology, 41, 711–722.

Tronick, E. Z., Morelli, G. A., & Ivey, P. K. (1992). The Efe forager infant and toddler’s pattern of social relationships: Multiple and simultaneous. Developmental Psychology, 28, 568–577.

Volling, B. L., McElwain, N. L., & Miller, A. L. (2002). Emotion regulation in context: The jealousy complex between young siblings and its relations with child and family characteristics. Child Development, 73, 581–600.

Walden, T. (1991). Infant social referencing. In J. Garber & K. Dodge (Eds.), The development of emotional regulation and dysregulation (pp. 69–88). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Watamura, S. E., Donzella, B., Alwin, J., & Gunnar, M. R. (2003). Morning to afternoon increases in cortisol concentration for infants and toddlers at child care: Age differences and behavioral correlates. Child Development, 74, 1006–1020.

Wellman, H. M., & Lempers, J. D. (1977). The naturalistic communicative abilities of two-year-olds. Child Development, 48, 1052–1057.